Advertisement
Review Article| Volume 94, P165-175, January 2022

Download started.

Ok

Shame in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizure: A narrative review

  • Author Footnotes
    ♦ Given his role as Editor in Chief, Markus Reuber had no involvement in the peer-review of this article and has no access to information regarding its peer-review. Full responsibility for the editorial process for this article was delegated to Christoph Helmstaedter.
    M Reuber
    Correspondence
    Corresponding author.
    Footnotes
    ♦ Given his role as Editor in Chief, Markus Reuber had no involvement in the peer-review of this article and has no access to information regarding its peer-review. Full responsibility for the editorial process for this article was delegated to Christoph Helmstaedter.
    Affiliations
    Academic Neurology Unit, University of Sheffield, Royal Hallamshire Hospital, Glossop Road, Sheffield, S10 2JF, United Kingdom
    Search for articles by this author
  • Nicole A. Roberts
    Affiliations
    School of Social and Behavioral Sciences, Arizona State University, Phoenix, AZ, USA
    Search for articles by this author
  • Liat Levita
    Affiliations
    Department of Psychology, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK
    Search for articles by this author
  • Cordelia Gray
    Affiliations
    Specialist Psychotherapist, Neurology Psychotherapy Service, Sheffield Teaching Hospital, Academic Neurology Unit, University of Sheffield, Sheffield, UK
    Search for articles by this author
  • Lorna Myers
    Affiliations
    Director, Northeast Regional Epilepsy Group, New York, United States
    Search for articles by this author
  • Author Footnotes
    ♦ Given his role as Editor in Chief, Markus Reuber had no involvement in the peer-review of this article and has no access to information regarding its peer-review. Full responsibility for the editorial process for this article was delegated to Christoph Helmstaedter.
Open ArchivePublished:October 25, 2021DOI:https://doi.org/10.1016/j.seizure.2021.10.017

      Highlights

      • PNES have been linked to dysregulated emotions and arousal.
      • Chronic shame, shame proneness and dysregulation are key problems in PNES.
      • Shame may be a predisposing, precipitating, perpetuating or trigger factor.
      • Shame may explain much of the phenomenology of PNES.
      • There is overlap of the neurobiology of shame and PNES.

      Abstract

      Psychogenic Nonepileptic Seizures (PNES) have been linked to dysregulated emotions and arousal. However, the question which emotions may be most relevant has received much less attention. In this multidisciplinary narrative review, we argue that the self-conscious emotion of shame is likely to be of particular importance for PNES. We summarize current concepts of the development of shame processing and its relationship with other emotional states. We demonstrate the potential of acute shame to cause a sudden disruption of normal cognitive function and trigger powerful behavioral, cognitive, physiological and secondary emotional responses which closely resemble key components of PNES. These responses may lead to the development of shame avoidance strategies which can become disabling in themselves. We discuss how excessive shame proneness and shame dysregulation are linked to several psychopathologies often associated with PNES (including depression and PTSD) and how they may predispose to, precipitate and perpetuate PNES disorders, not least by interacting with stigma. We consider current knowledge of the neurobiological underpinnings of shame and PNES. We explore how shame could be the link between PNES and a heterogeneous range of possible etiological factors, and how it may link historical aversive experiences with individual PNES events occurring much later and without apparent external trigger. We argue that, in view of the potential direct links between shame and PNES, the well-documented associations of shame with common comorbidities of this seizure disorder and the well-characterized relationship between chronic shame and stigma, there is a compelling case to pay greater attention to shame in relation to PNES. Its role in the treatment of patients with PNES is discussed in a separate, linked review incorporating case vignettes to highlight the complex interactions of different but interlinked shame-related issues in individual patients

      Keywords

      1. Introduction

      An emotion is the subjective experience of a mental state that directs our attention, guides our actions and is often accompanied by physiological and behavioral changes [
      • Levenson R.W.
      Human emotions: a functional view.
      ,
      • Barrett L.F.
      • Mesquita B.
      • Ochsner K.N.
      • Gross J.J.
      The experience of emotion.
      ,
      • Levenson R.W.
      • Soto J.
      • Pole N.
      Emotion, biology, and culture.
      ]. Traditional ideas of conversion and dissociation and more recent explanatory models based on emotion processing have linked Psychogenic Nonepileptic Seizures (PNES) closely with emotions [
      • Williams I.A.
      • Levita L.
      • Reuber M.
      Emotion dysregulation in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a systematic review based on the extended process model.
      ]. In support of these theories, experimental studies have demonstrated abnormalities of emotion sensitivity, intensity, perception, tolerance, regulation and baseline arousal when comparing patients with PNES to those with epilepsy, healthy controls, or control groups with similar levels of trauma [
      • Brown R.J.
      • Reuber M.
      Psychological and psychiatric aspects of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES): a systematic review.
      ,
      • Krámská L.
      • Hrešková L.
      • Vojtěch Z.
      • Krámský D.
      • Myers L.
      Maladaptive emotional regulation in patients diagnosed with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES) compared with healthy volunteers.
      ,
      • Pick S.
      • Goldstein L.H.
      • Perez D.L.
      • Nicholson T.R.
      Emotional processing in functional neurological disorder: a review, biopsychosocial model and research agenda.
      ,
      • Roberts N.A.
      • Burleson M.H.
      • Weber D.J.
      • Larson A.
      • Sergeant K.
      • Devine M.J.
      • Wang N.C.
      Emotion in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: responses to affective pictures.
      ]. Clinical research has highlighted the overlap of manifestations of PNES with symptoms of arousal [
      • Rawlings G.H.
      • Jamnadas-Khoda J.
      • Broadhurst M.
      • Grünewald R.A.
      • Howell S.J.
      • Koepp M.
      • et al.
      Panic symptoms in transient loss of consciousness: frequency and diagnostic value in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures, epilepsy and syncope.
      ], and documented high levels of correlation of anxiety and avoidance as well as avoidance and seizure frequency in patients with PNES [
      • Dimaro L.V.
      • Dawson D.L.
      • Roberts N.A.
      • Brown I.
      • Moghaddam N.G.
      • Reuber M.
      Anxiety and avoidance in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: the role of implicit and explicit anxiety.
      ].
      While observations like these firmly link dysregulated emotions and arousal with PNES, the question which emotions may be most relevant has received much less attention. In this article, we will argue that the self-conscious emotion of shame is likely to be of particular importance for PNES. While focussing on shame, we will also mention other self-conscious emotions such as guilt and embarrassment, although, for reasons discussed below, despite considerable subjective and neurobiological overlap with shame, these emotional states are likely to be less relevant in this clinical context.
      We do not claim that shame is a necessary or sufficient factor in all PNES disorders or that our impression that PNES are commonly associated with shame is a novel discovery [
      • Griffith J.L.
      • Polles A.
      • Griffith M.E.
      Pseudoseizures, families, and unspeakable dilemmas.
      ,
      • Hazard P.
      Freud's Teaching on Shame.
      ]. However, given that there are few emotional states that are as comprehensively and deeply debilitating as shame, we argue that shame often makes a particularly important and – so far – relatively neglected contribution to PNES.
      We note how the link between clinical symptoms and shame has been elucidated much more in relation to common PNES comorbidities such as posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) [
      • López-Castro T.
      • Saraiya T.
      • Zumberg-Smith K.
      • Dambreville N.
      Association Between Shame and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: a Meta-Analysis.
      ,
      • Robinaugh D.J.
      • McNally R.J.
      Autobiographical memory for shame or guilt provoking events: association with psychological symptoms.
      ], borderline personality disorder [
      • Rüsch N.
      • Lieb K.
      • Göttler I.
      • Hermann C.
      • Schramm E.
      • Richter H.
      • Bohus M.
      Shame and implicit self-concept in women with borderline personality disorder.
      ,
      • Peters J.R.
      • Geiger P.J.
      Borderline personality disorder and self-conscious affect: too much shame but not enough guilt?.
      ], obsessive compulsive related disorders [
      • Hennig-Fast K.
      • Michl P.
      • Müller J.
      • Niedermeier N.
      • Coates U.
      • Müller N.
      • Meindl T.
      Obsessive-compulsive disorder–A question of conscience? An fMRI study of behavioural and neurofunctional correlates of shame and guilt.
      ,
      • Weingarden H.
      • Renshaw K.D.
      Shame in the obsessive compulsive related disorders: a conceptual review.
      ] and depression [
      • Kim S.
      • Thibodeau R.
      • Jorgensen R.S.
      Shame, guilt, and depressive symptoms: a meta-analytic review.
      ], than it has in relation to PNES. It therefore strikes us that a discussion of the relationship between shame and PNES could provide a useful impetus to researchers and clinicians working in this field.
      Importantly, we do not think that our call to pay more attention to shame means that our conceptual models of PNES have to be thrown overboard. Knowledge about the origins, development and effects of this emotion can easily be fitted into current biopsychosocial explanatory approaches and the ‘Integrative Cognitive Model’ of PNES [
      • Brown R.J.
      • Reuber M.
      Towards an integrative theory of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES).
      ,
      • Reuber M.
      The etiology of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: toward a biopsychosocial model.
      ]. In fact, we will show how a fuller appreciation of shame can enhance these models and contribute to linking concepts, which focus on cognitive, emotional and physiological processes within one individual with PNES to the social and interpersonal environment in which these seizures arise. Shame can also help us understand how aversive experiences in early life may be linked to PNES disorders manifesting much later, or how recent stressors may trigger a PNES without externally perceptible triggers hours, days or weeks later.

      2. Emotions: definitions and conceptual issues

      Researchers have recognized an increasing number of more or less clearly separable emotions or “affective states” [
      • Cowen A.S.
      • Keltner D.
      Self-report captures 27 distinct categories of emotion bridged by continuous gradients.
      ]. These states can be categorized in a number of ways, such as on a continuum of valence (how negative or positive the experience feels) and arousal (how ‘strong’ the experience feels), or as discrete emotions (e.g., basic emotions such as happiness, anger, fear). ‘Social’ or ‘self-conscious’ emotions, including shame, guilt, and embarrassment (as well as pride) are named as such because they require knowledge of the self and facilitate the maintenance of social goals, such as sustaining functioning of individuals within a group [
      • Tracy J.L.
      • Robins R.W.
      Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: a theoretical model.
      ]. For similar reasons, shame, and guilt (along with certain forms of disgust) have also been described as ‘moral’ emotions [
      • Tangney J.P.
      • Stuewig J.
      • Mashek D.J.
      Moral emotions and moral behavior.
      ].

      2.1 Self-conscious emotions

      An appraisal-based process model developed by Tracy and Robins suggests that experiencing any kind of self-conscious emotion requires focusing attention on public or private aspects of the self; appraising the eliciting event as relevant to one's personal goals; and attributing the cause of the event to internal factors that result in blaming or crediting the self [
      • Tracy J.L.
      • Robins R.W.
      Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: a theoretical model.
      ]. For instance, failing an exam could cause feelings of shame or guilt if it was appraised as relevant to the person's identity and attributed to an internal failing, or feelings of anger, fear, or sadness if it was appraised as having an external cause.
      This appraisal model draws on attribution theory (i.e., the causes people attribute to events). According to the model, the determination whether guilt or shame will be experienced in relation to an event requires a second level of appraisal: Whereas a negative event perceived to be due to internal, stable, uncontrollable and global causes (eg. ‘‘it happened because I'm a bad person’’) would instill shame, an event attributed to internal, unstable, controllable and specific causes (‘‘It happened because I did a bad thing’’) causes guilt. In this context ‘stable’ refers to a part of the person that cannot change, ‘global’ to something likely to apply across different situations and ‘uncontrollable’ to something which is not an inherent characteristic of the person.
      Put differently, whereas guilt is related to feeling bad about a particular act or omission, shame relates to the person one is. This understanding is in keeping with the established view that shame is a more aversive and potentially destructive state of self-criticism than guilt. It is consistent with the experience that shame can harm an individual's feeling of self-worth to such an extent that it only leaves them with the option of hiding away from their own inadequacy or of externalizing their perceived inadequacy through anger or hostility towards others [
      • Leach C.W.
      Understanding shame and guilt.
      ].
      This model accommodates the view and evidence that there is considerable overlap between shame and guilt and that it can be challenging to identify when someone is experiencing guilt versus shame [e.g.,
      • Ferguson T.J.
      • Crowley S.L.
      Measure for measure: a multitrait-multimethod analysis of guilt and shame.
      ,
      • Eisenberg N.
      Emotion, regulation, and moral development.
      ,
      • Cândea D.M.
      • Szentagotai-Tătar A.
      Shame-proneness, guilt-proneness and anxiety symptoms: a meta-analysis.
      ]. It is also consistent with the idea that shame is not invariably harmful or destructive. Indeed, while there is a large body of research linking chronic shame with “internalized” problems like depression, anxiety and low self-esteem, as well as “externalized” problems like hostility, aggression and anti-social behavior in children [
      • Mills R.S.L.
      • Hastings P.D.
      • Serbin L.A.
      • Stack D.M.
      • Abela J.R.Z.
      • Arbeau K.A.
      • et al.
      Depressogenic thinking and shame proneness in the development of internalizing problems.
      ,
      • Luby J.
      • Belden A.
      • Sullivan J.
      • Hayen R.
      • McCadney A.
      • Spitznagel E.
      Shame and guilt in preschool depression: evidence for elevations in self-conscious emotions in depression as early as age 3.
      ,
      • Muris P.
      • Meesters C.
      Small or big in the eyes of the other: on the developmental psychopathology of self-conscious emotions as shame, guilt, and pride.
      ] adolescents [
      • De Rubeis S.
      • Hollenstein T.
      Individual differences in shame and depressive symptoms during early adolescence.
      ,
      • Stuewig J.
      • Tangney J.P.
      • Kendall S.
      • Folk J.B.
      • Meyer C.R.
      • Dearing R.L.
      Children’s proneness to shame and guilt predict risky and illegal behaviors in young adulthood.
      ,
      • Tilghman-Osborne C.
      • Cole D.A.
      • Felton J.W.
      • Ciesla J.A.
      Relation of guilt, shame, behavioral and characterological self-blame to depressive symptoms in adolescents over time.
      ], and adults [
      • Fergus T.A.
      • Valentiner D.P.
      • McGrath P.B.
      • Jencius S.
      Shame- and guilt-proneness: relationships with anxiety disorder symptoms in a clinical sample.
      ,
      • Kim S.
      • Thibodeau R.
      • Jorgensen R.S.
      Shame, guilt, and depressive symptoms: a meta-analytic review.
      ,
      • Shahar G.
      Personality, shame, and the breakdown of social bonds: the voice of quantitative depression research.
      ,
      • Treeby M.
      • Bruno R.
      Shame and guilt-proneness: divergent implications for problematic alcohol use and drinking to cope with anxiety and depression symptomatology.
      ,
      • Woien S.
      • Ernst H.
      • Patock-Peckham J.
      • Nagoshi C.
      Validation of the TOSCA to measure shame and guilt.
      ], a theory-driven meta-analysis of 90 studies involving shame paradigms (N = 12,364) demonstrated that (similarly to guilt) shame can have positive consequences on thoughts or actions as long as failure or social image seem reparable. In contrast, negative consequences are likely when they are perceived as irredeemable [
      • Leach C.W.
      • Cidam A.
      When is shame linked to constructive approach orientation? A meta-analysis.
      ]. In keeping with this, a number of recent studies have shown that recalled or current episodes of shame lead to a greater desire for self-improvement, cooperative behavior, and a more supportive orientation towards the victims of one's perceived moral failure [
      • Wang L.
      • Pan H.
      • Zhang H.
      The effect of emotional intensity of shame on children’s prosocial behaviour.
      ,

      Gaspar, A. and M. Henriques, Driven by shame: how a negative emotion may lead to prosocial behaviour. In: Sara Graca da Silva (ed): New interdisciplinary landscapes in morality and emotion. London: Routledge, chapter 4, 2018.

      ,
      • de Hooge I.E.
      • Breugelmans S.M.
      • Wagemans F.M.A.
      • Zeelenberg M.
      The social side of shame: approach versus withdrawal.
      ].

      2.2 Shame regulation

      Whether emotions such as shame play protective or adaptive roles or are harmful depends not only on the emotional experience but also on timing, context, and how emotions are regulated. Emotion regulation describes a person's ability to respond and manage subjective, behavioral, and/or physiological responses, and includes seeking or avoiding emotion-eliciting situations [
      • Gross J.J.
      • Thompson R.A.
      Emotion regulation: conceptual foundations.
      ]. As with other emotions, more frequent or intense shame experiences are likely to require greater effort to regulate this response [
      • Dixon-Gordon K.L.
      • Aldao A.
      • De Los Reyes A.
      Emotion regulation in context: examining the spontaneous use of strategies across emotional intensity and type of emotion.
      ]. Although emotion experience and regulation are therefore linked, it helps to distinguish ‘emotion regulation’ from ‘emotion proneness’. More specifically, ‘shame proneness’ may be defined as a characteristic describing how likely an individual is to experience shame in response to a given situation [
      • Tangney J.P.
      • Wagner P.
      • Gramzow R.
      Proneness to shame, proneness to guilt, and psychopathology.
      ]. One would talk of ‘shame dysregulation’ if there was inadequate regulation of the behavioral, cognitive, physiological and secondary emotional responses to the perceived shame. In individuals with excessive shame proneness and deficient shame regulation, compensatory interpersonal behaviors may become dominant and start to interfere with daily functioning. In contrast, if feelings of shame are insufficiently elicited, transgressions will not be followed by corrective behaviors and may cause interpersonal problems.
      Schoenleber & Berenbaum [
      • Schoenleber M.
      • Berenbaum H.
      Shame regulation in personality pathology.
      ], drawing on work on shame and emotion regulation, identified three broad maladaptive shame regulation strategies (see Fig. 1): ‘prevention’ (circumventing shame situations/experiences), ‘escape’ (disengaging from shame-eliciting situations), and ‘aggression’ (self- or other-directed acts attempting to manage shame experiences). This model was tested and received empirical support in individuals with borderline personality disorder, a condition characterized by emotion dysregulation and insecure attachment, which is often comorbid with PNES [
      • Reuber M.
      • Pukrop R.
      • Bauer J.
      • Derfuss R.
      • Elger C.E.
      Multidimensional assessment of personality in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures.
      ,
      • Beghi M.
      • Negrini P.B.
      • Perin C.
      • Peroni F.
      • Magaudda A.
      • Cerri C.
      • et al.
      Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: so-called psychiatric comorbidity and underlying defense mechanisms.
      ]. All of the shame regulation activities included in this model are likely to be employed by patients with PNES. As argued below and illustrated in Fig. 1, PNES themselves usually involve a mixture of elements of escape and aggression (mostly self-directed, but also other-directed, typically in a passive-aggressive form).
      Fig. 1
      Fig. 1.Three broad maladaptive shame regulation strategies and their relation to PNES, where other-directed aggression is less prominent in PNES than self-directed aggression, as indicated by the lighter font (adapted from [
      • Schoenleber M.
      • Berenbaum H.
      Shame regulation in personality pathology.
      ]).

      3. Developmental aspects

      The fact that shame processing plays a key role for healthy personal and social development but that dysregulated shame is also closely linked with psychopathology and abnormal social functioning underlines the importance of the developmental trajectory of shame [
      • Mills R.S.L.
      Taking stock of the developmental literature on shame.
      ].

      3.1 Typical shame development

      Basic emotions emerge very early in development. For instance, happiness is expressed at 6 weeks by the smile evoked by a human face. Self-conscious emotions, however, and initial manifestations of guilt-motivated behavior (e.g., reparations to mend transgressions; confessions for harmful acts, feeling badly about oneself, avoidance of the person who was transgressed upon) or shame reactions are not observed until 18–24 months of age [
      • Barrett K.C.
      • Zahn-waxler C.
      • Cole P.M.
      Avoiders vs. Amenders: implications for the investigation of guilt and shame during Toddlerhood?.
      ,
      • Cole P.M.
      • Barrett K.C.
      • Zahn-Waxler C.
      Emotion displays in two-year-olds during mishaps.
      ,
      • Kochanska G.
      • DeVet K.
      • Goldman M.
      • Murray K.
      • Putnam S.P.
      Maternal reports of conscience development and temperament in young children.
      ]. These emotions can only occur once toddlers have become aware of themselves as separate individuals.
      The self-concept [
      • Barrett K.C.
      • Zahn-waxler C.
      • Cole P.M.
      Avoiders vs. Amenders: implications for the investigation of guilt and shame during Toddlerhood?.
      ,
      • Lewis M.
      Self-conscious emotions: embarrassment, pride,shame, and guilt.
      ,
      • Barrett K.C.
      A functionalist perspective to the development of emotions.
      ] matures into a more stable set of self-representations around the age of 3 [
      • Eisenberg N.
      • Fabes R.A.
      • Guthrie I.K.
      • Reiser M.
      Dispositional emotionality and regulation: their role in predicting quality of social functioning.
      ,
      • Kochanska G.
      • Gross J.N.
      • Lin M.H.
      • Nichols K.E.
      Guilt in young children: development, determinants, and relations with a broader system of standards.
      ]. In addition to the child's ability to evaluate the self and to differentiate between themselves and others, the processes underpinning the experience of shame and guilt depend on the capacity to empathize with others, and to understand social and moral standards [
      • Muris P.
      • Meesters C.
      Small or big in the eyes of the other: on the developmental psychopathology of self-conscious emotions as shame, guilt, and pride.
      ,
      • Denham S.
      • Bassett H.
      • Wyatt T.
      The socialization of emotional competence.
      ,
      • Eisenberg N.
      • Fabes R.A.
      • Spinrad T.L.
      Handbook of child psychology: volume 3.Social, emotional, and personality development.
      ]. Apart from self-awareness, the most important element for the normal development and processing of self-conscious emotions in young children is parental or caregiver input [
      • Muris P.
      • Meesters C.
      Small or big in the eyes of the other: on the developmental psychopathology of self-conscious emotions as shame, guilt, and pride.
      ]. The experience of supportive parents tolerating missteps and perceived personal defects, and of parents providing comfort when children share such guilt- or shame-inducing experiences or perceptions, helps children to develop healthy levels of shame tolerance, regulation and management. In contrast, as described below, the lack of an appropriate parental response is likely to stifle the development of adequate shame management and regulation mechanisms, potentially leading to pervasive or hypersensitive shame reactions (i.e. excessive shame proneness), and/or avoidance or mislabeling of shame states.
      As children grow older, observable shame responses become similar to those seen in adults. They involve cessation of movement, impassivity, and the child appearing to experience a disruption of behavior and thought processes [
      • Denham S.
      • Bassett H.
      • Wyatt T.
      The socialization of emotional competence.
      ,
      • Lewis M.
      Shame: the exposed self.
      ]. These responses become more pronounced during the preschool period as children's cognitive, language, social development and awareness advance and evolve further to late adolescence [
      • Eisenberg N.
      • Fabes R.A.
      • Spinrad T.L.
      Handbook of child psychology: volume 3.Social, emotional, and personality development.
      ,
      • Berti A.
      • Garattoni C.
      • Venturini B.
      The Understanding of sadness, guilt, and shame in 5-, 7-, and 9-year-old children.
      ]. In keeping with this, shame increases during adolescence. However, as individuals mature into adulthood they become better able to experience psychologically adaptive self-conscious emotions (such as guilt and authentic pride) and less prone to experiencing psychologically maladaptive ones (such as shame and hubristic pride) [
      • Orth U.
      • Robins R.W.
      • Soto C.J.
      Tracking the trajectory of shame, guilt, and pride across the life span.
      ].

      3.3 Atypical shame development: determinants and consequences

      The level of shame proneness and tolerance as well as the triggers which elicit it are shaped by individual experiences, a person's sociocultural environment, and genetic factors [
      • Wong Y.
      • Tsai J.
      Cultural models of shame and guilt.
      ]. In one study of healthy adolescents aged 14–17 years (N = 271) the association between self-reported early traumatic events and proneness to shame and guilt was moderated by the BDNF Val66Met status: trauma intensity was positively associated with guilt-proneness, but only in carriers of the low-expressing Met-allele of this gene [
      • Szentágotai-Tătar A.
      • Chiș A.
      • Vulturar R.
      • Dobrean A.
      • Cândea D.M.
      • Miu A.C.
      Shame and guilt-proneness in adolescents: gene-environment interactions.
      ]. The finding that levels of shame and guilt are higher in non-clinical and clinical samples of girls compared to boys could also be related to genetic factors (or gender-dependent gene-environment interaction), but experiential or sociocultural factors are also likely to play important roles [
      • Muris P.
      • Meesters C.
      • Heijmans J.
      • van Hulten S.
      • Kaanen L.
      • Oerlemans B.
      • Tielemans T.
      Lack of guilt, guilt, and shame: a multi-informant study on the relations between self-conscious emotions and psychopathology in clinically referred children and adolescents.
      ,
      • Else-Quest N.M.
      • Higgins A.
      • Allison C.
      • Morton L.C.
      Gender differences in self-conscious emotional experience: a meta-analysis.
      ].
      Poor parenting is the best studied determinant of excessive feelings of shame. Adults who retrospectively report receiving low parental care and greater rejection are more prone to feel shame [
      • Claesson K.
      • Sohlberg S.
      Internalized shame and early interactions characterized by indifference, abandonment and rejection: replicated findings.
      ,
      • Gilbert P.
      • Allan S.
      • Goss K.
      Parental representations, shame, interpersonal problems, and vulnerability to psychopathology.
      ,
      • Lutwak N.
      • Ferrari J.
      Understanding shame in adults: retrospective perceptions of parental-bonding during childhood.
      ]. Studies with toddlers [
      • Kelley S.A.
      • Brownell C.A.
      • Campbell S.B.
      Mastery motivation and self-evaluative affect in toddlers: longitudinal relations with maternal behavior.
      ] and older children [
      • Han D.H.
      • Kim S.M.
      • Lee Y.S.
      • Renshaw P.F.
      The effect of family therapy on the changes in the severity of on-line game play and brain activity in adolescents with on-line game addiction.
      ,
      • Stuewig J.
      • McCloskey L.A.
      The relation of child maltreatment to shame and guilt among adolescents: psychological routes to depression and delinquency.
      ] also suggest that children are at increased risk of shame expression if they have rejecting parents who provide little positive feedback. In a study on shame in young girls (aged 3–5), maternal and paternal authoritarian parenting predicted girls’ shame responses [
      • Mills R.S.L.
      Possible antecedents and developmental implications of shame in young girls.
      ]. Mills and colleagues also found that maternal shaming predicted shame responses in children from preschool to school age [
      • Mills R.S.L.
      • Arbeau K.A.
      • Lall D.I.K.
      • De Jaeger A.E.
      Parenting and child characteristics in the prediction of shame in early and middle childhood.
      ]. In addition, a longitudinal study has found that a history of paternal but not maternal depression and permissive parenting (measured when their children were 3 years old) predicted expression of shame and guilt by these children at the age of 6 [
      • Parisette-Sparks A.
      • Bufferd S.J.
      • Klein D.N.
      Parental predictors of children's shame and guilt at age 6 in a multimethod, longitudinal study.
      ].
      Traumatic experiences in early life may compound the effects of unsupportive parenting and are often difficult to separate from it. This may be relevant in patients with PNES where studies have shown that sexual abuse is often associated with emotional neglect and broader family dysfunction [
      • Salmon P.
      • Al-Marzooqi S.M.
      • Baker G.
      • Reilly J.
      Childhood family dysfunction and associated abuse in patients with nonepileptic seizures: towards a causal model.
      ][
      • Dong M.
      • Anda R.F.
      • Dube S.R.
      • Giles W.H.
      • Felitti V.J.
      The relationship of exposure to childhood sexual abuse to other forms of abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction during childhood.
      ,
      • Dong M.
      • Anda R.F.
      • Felitti V.J.
      • Dube S.R.
      • Williamson D.F.
      • Thompson T.J.
      • et al.
      The interrelatedness of multiple forms of childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction.
      ,
      • Dobson K.S.
      • McLarnon M.J.W.
      • Pandya K.
      • Pusch D.
      A latent profile analysis of adverse childhood experiences and adult health in a community sample.
      ].
      Of course, the developmental aspects of shame perception need to be understood in the context of the development of an individual's broader emotion regulation capacity [
      • Sanchis-Sanchis A.
      • Grau M.D.
      • Moliner A.-.R.
      • Morales-Murillo C.P.
      Effects of age and gender in emotion regulation of children and adolescents.
      ,
      • Cole P.M.
      Moving ahead in the study of the development of emotion regulation.
      ]. Indeed, Szentágotai-Tătar & Miu [
      • Szentágotai-Tătar A.
      • Miu A.C.
      Individual differences in emotion regulation, childhood trauma and proneness to shame and guilt in adolescence.
      ] found that dysfunctional shame-proneness during adolescence was closely related to a broader pattern of maladaptive emotion regulation and to subsequent decreases in prosocial behavior.
      The key role of dysregulated shame in the generation of manifestations of Borderline Personality Disorder has already been discussed (see Section 2.2). An inherited or acquired predisposition to excessive shame has also been linked to Posttraumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD), major depressive disorder (MDD), and obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD) [
      • Saraiya T.
      • Lopez-Castro T.
      Ashamed and afraid: a scoping review of the role of shame in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD).
      ,
      • Weingarden H.
      • Renshaw K.D.
      Shame in the obsessive compulsive related disorders: a conceptual review.
      ,
      • Zahn R.
      • Lythe K.E.
      • Gethin J.A.
      • Green S.
      • Deakin J.F.W.
      • Young A.H.
      • et al.
      The role of self-blame and worthlessness in the psychopathology of major depressive disorder.
      ,
      • Pulcu E.
      • Lythe K.
      • Elliott R.
      • Green S.
      • Moll J.
      • Deakin J.F.
      • Zahn R.
      Increased amygdala response to shame in remitted major depressive disorder.
      ]. Similarly recurrent shame experiences, high shame proneness and inadequate shame regulation may lead to more frequent, prolonged or abnormally intense shame experiences in individuals with PNES, which could interact with anxious escape tendencies to promote excessively avoidant behaviors and a dynamic of increasing shame dysregulation. Depression research suggests that children excessively prone to shame (e.g., due to genetic factors and/or early life experiences) can enter self-reinforcing states of shame as they “focus attention on images of devaluation and rejection by others” [29, p.195]. Continual experiences of shame may reinforce these images and feelings over time, contributing to “feelings of helplessness and hopelessness about the self”, increasing the risk of subsequent mood or behavioral disturbance [29, p.195]. Conceivably, shame-related erosion of the self could contribute to the lack of control patients with PNES report having over their seizures and to their very external health-related locus of control [
      • Whitehead K.
      • Kandler R.
      • Reuber M.
      Patients' and neurologists' perception of epilepsy and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
      ,
      • Stone J.
      • Binzer M.
      • Sharpe M.
      Illness beliefs and locus of control: a comparison of patients with pseudoseizures and epilepsy.
      ].

      4. Neurobiological underpinnings of shame

      4.1 Brain regions associated with shame

      In adults, a number of studies have examined neurobiological underpinnings of shame, most often alongside other self-conscious emotions - guilt and embarrassment; [
      • Finger E.C.
      • Marsh A.A.
      • Kamel N.
      • Mitchell D.G.
      • Blair J.R.
      Caught in the act: the impact of audience on the neural response to morally and socially inappropriate behavior.
      ,
      • Wagner U.
      • N'Diaye K.
      • Ethofer T.
      • Vuilleumier P.
      Guilt-specific processing in the prefrontal cortex.
      ], other negative emotions such as sadness [
      • Lindquist K.A.
      • Wager T.D.
      • Kober H.
      • Bliss-Moreau E.
      • Barrett L.F.
      The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review.
      ], or positive self-conscious emotions such as pride [
      • Roth L.
      • Kaffenberger T.
      • Herwig U.
      • Brühl A.B.
      Brain activation associated with pride and shame.
      ]. In a review of 21 neuroimaging studies examining shame, guilt, and/or embarrassment [
      • Bastin C.
      • Harrison B.J.
      • Davey C.G.
      • Moll J.
      • Whittle S.
      Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: a systematic review.
      ], five specifically examined shame [
      • Pulcu E.
      • Lythe K.
      • Elliott R.
      • Green S.
      • Moll J.
      • Deakin J.F.
      • Zahn R.
      Increased amygdala response to shame in remitted major depressive disorder.
      ,
      • Finger E.C.
      • Marsh A.A.
      • Kamel N.
      • Mitchell D.G.
      • Blair J.R.
      Caught in the act: the impact of audience on the neural response to morally and socially inappropriate behavior.
      ,
      • Wagner U.
      • N'Diaye K.
      • Ethofer T.
      • Vuilleumier P.
      Guilt-specific processing in the prefrontal cortex.
      ,
      • Roth L.
      • Kaffenberger T.
      • Herwig U.
      • Brühl A.B.
      Brain activation associated with pride and shame.
      ,
      • Michl P.
      • Meindl T.
      • Meister F.
      • Born C.
      • Engel R.R.
      • Reiser M.
      • et al.
      Neurobiological underpinnings of shame and guilt: a pilot fMRI study.
      ]. Neural activation unique to shame was observed in the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, posterior cingulate cortex, and sensorimotor cortex [
      • Bastin C.
      • Harrison B.J.
      • Davey C.G.
      • Moll J.
      • Whittle S.
      Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: a systematic review.
      ]. Differences in neural activation were clearer when shame was compared with neutral conditions than in comparisons of shame and “control” emotions [
      • Bastin C.
      • Harrison B.J.
      • Davey C.G.
      • Moll J.
      • Whittle S.
      Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: a systematic review.
      ]. This is not surprising given the shared processes and neural substrates underlying emotional experience, such as language, conceptualization, and core affect [
      • Lindquist K.A.
      • Wager T.D.
      • Kober H.
      • Bliss-Moreau E.
      • Barrett L.F.
      The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review.
      ].
      In particular, one would expect overlap between shame and guilt. As described earlier, self-referential processing is a hallmark of both of these emotions; they require evaluation of or reflection on one's own behavior, as well as perspective-taking of another's state (“theory of mind”), and the integration of the two. As Zhu et al. [
      • Zhu R.
      • Feng C.
      • Zhang S.
      • Mai X.
      • Liu C.
      Differentiating guilt and shame in an interpersonal context with univariate activation and multivariate pattern analyses.
      ] summarize, these processes are associated with activation of particular brain regions, specifically the anterior and posterior cingulate cortex for self-referential processing; superior temporal sulcus and temporo-parietal junction (TPJ) for theory of mind; and dorsomedial prefrontal cortex (dmPFC) for their integration, as well as anterior insular cortex and amygdala for salience and emotional processing. Indeed, during an interpersonal decision-making task designed to evoke shame or guilt both emotions were associated with greater (left) anterior insula and (bilateral) dmPFC activation, compared with experiences of happiness [
      • Zhu R.
      • Feng C.
      • Zhang S.
      • Mai X.
      • Liu C.
      Differentiating guilt and shame in an interpersonal context with univariate activation and multivariate pattern analyses.
      ]. Comparing shame and happiness, shame showed greater activation in these regions than happiness - but guilt did as well. Further, Zhu et al. (2019) [
      • Zhu R.
      • Feng C.
      • Zhang S.
      • Mai X.
      • Liu C.
      Differentiating guilt and shame in an interpersonal context with univariate activation and multivariate pattern analyses.
      ] found support for their prediction that guilt would show greater activation than shame in regions related to perspective-taking (left supramarginal gyrus, right TPJ) and cognitive control (right vlPFC/OFC and right dlPFC). Given that the right TPJ has been implicated in generating a sense of agency [
      • Zito G.A.
      • Wiest R.
      • Aybek S.
      Neural correlates of sense of agency in motor control: a neuroimaging meta-analysis.
      ], it is possible that guilt responses – albeit subjectively uncomfortable – may coincide with greater feelings of agency, whereas shame-proneness may not yield this benefit.
      Even if neural activation does not consistently and widely distinguish shame from other emotions, the neurobiological changes that occur during shame experience are noteworthy. For example, in Bastin et al.’s [
      • Bastin C.
      • Harrison B.J.
      • Davey C.G.
      • Moll J.
      • Whittle S.
      Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: a systematic review.
      ] review, anterior insular cortex was activated in all (100%) instances of shame elicitation, compared with neutral elicitation. The anterior insula facilitates mapping of interoceptive signals to conscious awareness and contributes to the experience of feelings [
      • Craig A.D.
      How do you feel — Now? The anterior insula and human awareness.
      ,
      • Craig A.D.
      Significance of the insula for the evolution of human awareness of feelings from the body.
      ,
      • Damasio A.
      • Carvalho G.B.
      The nature of feelings: evolutionary and neurobiological origins.
      ]. Thus, shame may be linked to the essence of “self” in a very real way, which perhaps is what leads its experience to be so profound and self-defining.
      Given the role of self-referential processing in shame as described above, shame-associated neural activation patterns could create a more negative (and shame-oriented) sense of self. Among those with remitted MDD, during a scripted (hypothetical scenario) imagery task, shame compared to guilt was associated with increased activation in the amygdala and posterior insula, whereas this was not the case among non-depressed controls [
      • Pulcu E.
      • Lythe K.
      • Elliott R.
      • Green S.
      • Moll J.
      • Deakin J.F.W.
      • Zahn R.
      Increased amygdala response to shame in remitted major depressive disorder.
      ]. This underscores the potential for shame to be integrated with one's neurobiology in ways that “outlast” overt signs and symptoms of a clinical syndrome such as MDD and could represent one particular link between shame-inducing traumas in early life and the emergence of PNES in adolescence or adulthood. Other clinical disorders offer further examples of how differences in neural processing associated with psychopathology may predispose to (or result from) pervasive shame states. Among women with PTSD, self-referential processing shows a different neural activation pattern than in controls - right amygdala activation, rather than the typical anterior cingulate cortex activation (perigenual region) – in response to positive self-descriptors (which the authors speculate might be beneficial in this context) [
      • Frewen P.A.
      • Dozois D.J.A.
      • Neufeld R.W.J.
      • Densmore M.
      • Stevens T.K.
      • Lanius R.A.
      Self-referential processing in women with PTSD: affective and neural response.
      ]. Individuals with OCD not only endorse elevated, more pervasive shame on self-report measures than healthy controls, but also show different patterns of neural activation in response to shame- and guilt-inductions using sentences – such as greater parahippocampal gyrus, middle temporal gyrus, and hypothalamic activation to shame versus neutral conditions, as opposed to greater fronto-parietal activation for controls – perhaps suggesting heightened encoding and elaboration (“vividness”) of shame in OCD, versus typical affect regulation and processing of shame stimuli [
      • Hennig-Fast K.
      • Michl P.
      • Müller J.
      • Niedermeier N.
      • Coates U.
      • Müller N.
      • Meindl T.
      Obsessive-compulsive disorder–A question of conscience? An fMRI study of behavioural and neurofunctional correlates of shame and guilt.
      ].

      4.2 Shame and activation of the autonomic nervous system

      Studies of autonomic nervous system indicators of emotional arousal provide further insights into the neurobiology of shame. Respiratory sinus arrhythmia (RSA), or high-frequency heart rate variability, has received particular attention as an indicator of parasympathetic nervous system engagement (cardiac vagal control). Higher resting state RSA is associated with calm, regulated mood states [Reviewed in,
      • Balzarotti S.
      • Biassoni F.
      • Colombo B.
      • Ciceri M.R.
      Cardiac vagal control as a marker of emotion regulation in healthy adults: a review.
      ]. Among healthy controls, greater RSA reactivity or “vagal flexibility” – higher baseline RSA followed by greater RSA decreases (vagal withdrawal) to a stressful speech task – was associated with greater self-reports of shame in response to negative social evaluation, yet less shame in response to positive feedback [
      • Muhtadie L.
      • Koslov K.
      • Akinola M.
      • Mendes W.B.
      Vagal flexibility: a physiological predictor of social sensitivity.
      ]. This echoes the perspective that shame in itself is not problematic if it is linked to a specific, appropriate context and is relatively short-lived, whereas an undifferentiated and unrelenting experience of shame is potentially more harmful. In a study of females with PTSD due to interpersonal trauma (physical and/or sexual assault/abuse), shame proneness (self-reported shame during the past month) and current reports of shame/guilt (combined across ratings of these two emotions) were associated with lower RSA during a resting baseline, trauma-related images (10 slides presented 5 s each), and a 2-minute recovery period after trauma image-viewing. Further, shame/guilt but not fear/anxiety was associated with lower RSA during the post-trauma-image recovery period [
      • Freed S.
      • D’Andrea W.
      Autonomic arousal and emotion in victims of interpersonal violence: shame proneness but not anxiety predicts vagal tone.
      ]. This suggests that shame/guilt experiences in response to traumatic stress reminders may have longer-lasting physiological effects than other emotions. It is worth noting that shame-related alterations of heart rate variability may be associated with other biological factors capable of contributing to poor long-term physical and mental health, as threats to the “social self” resulting in shame are associated with greater HPA (cortisol) and proinflammatory cytokine responses [
      • Dickerson S.S.
      • Gruenewald T.L.
      • Kemeny M.E.
      When the social self is threatened: shame, physiology, and health.
      ].

      5. Shame and PNES

      In the next section of our review, we list arguments supporting the idea of a close link between PNES and shame. Any relationship between a putative emotional process and its visible manifestations – such as how shame might be associated with a seizure-like condition – is undoubtedly complex. We will argue that shame-inducing stimuli may not only trigger and shape individual seizures but that heightened shame proneness and shame dysregulation contribute to the etiology of PNES disorders as predisposing, precipitating or perpetuating factors (see Fig. 2).
      Fig. 2
      Fig. 2.Model of how shaming experiences, shame proneness, shame dysregulation and stigma may contribute to the etiology of PNES disorders as predisposing, precipitating or perpetuating factors (adapted from
      [
      • Reuber M.
      The etiology of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: toward a biopsychosocial model.
      ]
      ).
      Shame-stimulus processing may not only link trauma in early life with PNES in later life but shame-inducing memories could also explain the common temporal dissociation between acute stressors and the occurrence of PNES in a more reflective, apparently peaceful moment. In a specific PNES, shame-related involvement likely reflects a mixture of shame itself and the person's reaction to this intolerable emotional state (including dissociated awareness and rage).

      5.1 Phenomenology of PNES and shame

      PNES can present with a range of semiologies but the most common manifestations demonstrate at least some overlap with the behavioral changes associated with shame and its immediate processing in the form of self- or other aggression (or rage) and escape (see Fig. 1). The facial expressions of shame include downward gaze aversion and downward head movement [
      • Keltner D.
      • Buswell B.N.
      Evidence for the distinctness of embarrassment, shame, and guilt: a study of recalled antecedents and facial expressions of emotion.
      ,
      • Keltner D.
      Signs of appeasement: evidence for the distinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and shame.
      ,
      • Izard C.E.
      Human emotions. emotions, personality, and psychotherapy.
      ,
      • Widen S.C.
      • Christy A.M.
      • Hewett K.
      • Russell J.A.
      Do proposed facial expressions of contempt, shame, embarrassment, and compassion communicate the predicted emotion?.
      ]). The body is often lowered, shoulders slump; the upper torso seems to collapse [
      • Keltner D.
      Evidence for the distinctness of embarrassment, shame, and guilt: a study of recalled antecedents and facial expressions of emotion.
      ]. This corporeal behavior seems consistent with the shame-associated urge to vanish, hide or die to escape the intense discomfort associated with shame. The realization that one's own practical or moral standards have not been met results in the halting of behavior. This may include the disruption of thought processes and an inability to speak.
      In the best study of the observable semiology of PNES, two raters studied video recordings and looked for 22 signs in 145 PNES episodes collected from 52 patients [
      • Hubsch C.
      • Baumann C.
      • Hingray C.
      • Gospodaru N.
      • Vignal J.P.
      • Vespignani H.
      • Maillard L.
      Clinical classification of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures based on video-EEG analysis and automatic clustering.
      ]. The most commonly observed signs included axial immobility (72.3%), closed eyes (60.9%), tremor (43%) and sudden onset (41.4%). The mouth was often tightly closed. Other studies have demonstrated that 95% of patients with PNES exhibit impaired responsiveness in their seizures. Almost all patients with PNES report seizure-related memory gaps [
      • Bell W.L.
      • Park Y.D.
      • Thompson E.A.
      • Radtke R.A.
      Ictal cognitive assessment of partial seizures and pseudoseizures.
      ]. Vocalization is relatively infrequent in PNES - it was only observed in 18.6% of seizures examined in the study cited above [
      • Hubsch C.
      • Baumann C.
      • Hingray C.
      • Gospodaru N.
      • Vignal J.P.
      • Vespignani H.
      • Maillard L.
      Clinical classification of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures based on video-EEG analysis and automatic clustering.
      ].
      Many of the differences in the visible manifestations of acute shame and the semiology of PNES may be explained by the immediate processing of the initial shame-trigger in terms of self- and other-directed aggression and escape (dissociation). While assaults on others (such has biting, hitting or scratching) are an infrequent manifestation of PNES, auto-aggressive acts (such has hitting the self or seizure-related self-injury) are relatively common and could be manifestations of levels of hostility or aggression which have been found to be higher in those with PNES than healthy controls of patients with other somatoform disorders [114]. PNES may also have a passive aggressive component (for instance by punishing or controlling others [
      • Mökleby K.
      • Blomhoff S.
      • Malt U.F.
      • Dahlström A.
      • Tauböll E.
      • Gjerstad L.
      Psychiatric comorbidity and hostility in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures compared with somatoform disorders and healthy controls.
      ]).
      Similarly to the autonomic manifestations of acute shame, PNES are associated with a ‘fight / flight / freeze’ response characterized by a reduction in parasympathetic and an increase in sympathetic tone [
      • Freed S.
      • D’Andrea W.
      Autonomic arousal and emotion in victims of interpersonal violence: shame proneness but not anxiety predicts vagal tone.
      ,
      • Ponnusamy A.
      • Marques J.L.B.
      • Reuber M.
      Comparison of heart rate variability parameters during complex partial seizures and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
      ]. Like women with PTSD and elevated levels of shame proneness [
      • Freed S.
      • D’Andrea W.
      Autonomic arousal and emotion in victims of interpersonal violence: shame proneness but not anxiety predicts vagal tone.
      ], patients with PNES (at group level) are characterized by reduced RSA and chronic hyperarousal in the interictal state [
      • Ponnusamy A.
      • Marques J.L.
      • Reuber M.
      Heart rate variability measures as biomarkers in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: potential and limitations.
      ,
      • Bakvis P.
      • Spinhoven P.
      • Giltay E.J.
      • Kuyk J.
      • Edelbroek P.M.
      • Zitman F.G.
      • Roelofs K.
      Basal hypercortisolism and trauma in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
      ]. Although in one study RSA reactivity (change from baseline) did not differ between PNES and Trauma Controls while they relived a shameful memory, some individuals from the PNES group did not complete the task due to feeling overwhelmed, and therefore the greatest autonomic responders were likely excluded [
      • Roberts N.A.
      • Burleson M.H.
      • Torres D.L.
      • Parkhurst D.K.
      • Garrett R.
      • Mitchell L.B.
      • Wang N.C.
      Emotional reactivity as a vulnerability for psychogenic nonepileptic seizures? Responses while reliving specific emotions.
      ]. The process of dissociation of awareness may help individuals to switch from a hyperaroused ‘ictal’ state to one of recovery: studies of heart rate variability document physiological arousal before and during an episode (high sympathetic tone), and diminished arousal immediately afterward (an increase in vagal tone) [
      • Romigi A.
      • Ricciardo Rizzo G.
      • Izzi F.
      • Guerrisi M.
      • Caccamo M.
      • Testa F.
      • et al.
      Heart rate variability parameters during psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: comparison between patients with pure PNES and comorbid epilepsy.
      ,
      • van der Kruijs S.J.M.
      • Vonck K.E.J.
      • Langereis G.R.
      • Feijs L.M.G.
      • Bodde N.M.G.
      • Lazeron R.H.C.
      • et al.
      Autonomic nervous system functioning associated with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: analysis of heart rate variability.
      ]. These observations are in keeping with the interpretation that dissociative seizures are a kind of cognitive and emotional ‘reset’ which stops the processes that have elicited the seizure.
      Indeed, this switch may be one of the factors why some patients report ‘wilfully submitting’ to a PNES [
      • van der Kruijs S.J.
      • Vonck K.E.
      • Langereis G.R.
      • Feijs L.M.
      • Bodde N.M.
      • Lazeron R.H.
      • et al.
      Autonomic nervous system functioning associated with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: analysis of heart rate variability.
      ].
      While PNES may bring relief from a distressing emotional state in the short term, the very nature of most seizures is likely to achieve the opposite effect of a ‘normal’ shame response: whereas the behavioral withdrawal associated with feeling shame makes a person less conspicuous, the visual manifestations and interpersonal consequences of PNES are likely to draw attention to the person, their seizures and their underlying causes. This may well increase the shame experienced by patients with PNES and make subsequent PNES more likely. It could be one of the reasons why PNES often occur in clusters [
      • Baird G.L.
      • Harlow L.L.
      • Machan J.T.
      • Thomas D.
      • LaFrance Jr., W.C.
      Identifying seizure clusters in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
      ] and why chronic PNES disorders are often particularly difficult to treat.

      5.2 Shame as a trigger of PNES

      As mentioned above, in one study exploring the relationship between shame and PNES, when asked to relive a shameful memory, a subset of those with PNES were unable or unwilling to do so. In other participants shameful memories triggered a PNES episode; this did not happen with other emotions (anger, happiness), nor were any noteworthy behavioral responses to the evocation of a shameful mempry observed among clinical controls (e.g., asking to stop the task, removing the physiological sensors, leaving the room) [
      • Roberts N.A.
      • Burleson M.H.
      • Torres D.L.
      • Parkhurst D.K.
      • Garrett R.
      • Mitchell L.B.
      • Wang N.C.
      Emotional reactivity as a vulnerability for psychogenic nonepileptic seizures? Responses while reliving specific emotions.
      ]. While further research still has to confirm the shame-PNES and shame-avoidance-PNES relationship in patients’ daily lives, a relationship between shame, shame avoidance and PNES may be suggested by the observation of PNES during psychotherapy. Especially in early psychotherapy sessions, when, touching upon a traumatic and shameful memory, the patient may exhibit an almost immediate, visceral reaction, and go into a seizure [
      • Kemp S.
      • Graham C.D.
      • Chan R.
      • Kitchingman H.
      • Vickerman K.
      • Reuber M.
      The frequency and management of seizures during psychological treatment among patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures and epilepsy.
      ]. Additional evidence may emerge as therapy progresses and patients become better able to tolerate and verbalize their emotional discomfort without seizing [
      • Myers L.
      • Zandberg L.
      Prolonged exposure therapy for comorbid psychogenic nonepileptic seizures and posttraumatic stress disorder.
      ]. Often they can then describe the nature of their discomfort. As shame diminishes, PNES do as well.

      5.3 Trauma, adversity, neglect and shame in the etiology of PNES

      There is a well-documented association of PNES and trauma in early life, i.e., at a point in a child's development when the neurobiological and psychological mechanisms underpinning self-conscious emotions are evolving [
      • Reuber M.
      • Micoulaud-Franchi J.A.
      • Gülich E.
      • Bartolomei F.
      • McGonigal A.
      Comment ce que disent les patients peut nous renseigner sur leurs crises non épileptiques psychogènes [What the patient’s history tells us about their nonepileptic seizures].
      ,
      • Ludwig L.
      • Pasman J.A.
      • Nicholson T.
      • Aybek S.
      • David A.S.
      • Tuck S.
      • Stone J.
      Stressful life events and maltreatment in conversion (functional neurological) disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis of case-control studies.
      ,
      • Sharpe D.
      • Faye C.
      Non-epileptic seizures and child sexual abuse: a critical review of the literature.
      ]. Although difficult to study, quantify or compare in terms of its relevance in relation to other aetiological factors, emotional neglect during this developmental stage may be even more prevalent than trauma [
      • Holman N.
      • Kirkby A.
      • Duncan S.
      • Brown R.J.
      Adult attachment style and childhood interpersonal trauma in non-epileptic attack disorder.
      ,
      • Levita L.
      • Mayberry E.
      • Mehmood A.
      • Reuber M.
      Evaluation of LiNES: a new measure of trauma, negative affect, and relationship insecurity over the life span in persons with FND.
      ]. Excessive shame-proneness or the dysregulation of shame processing are plausible links between these developmental adversities and subsequent psychopathology including PNES, which may intersect with individual predisposing biological vulnerabilities as previously identified in stress-diathesis models of PNES [
      • Keynejad R.C.
      • Frodl T.
      • Kanaan R.
      • Pariante C.
      • Reuber M.
      • Nicholson T.R.
      Stress and functional neurological disorders: mechanistic insights.
      ]. As stated above, high levels of shame in early life are a risk factor for subsequent psychopathology.
      Patients with PNES also report high rates of trauma in adulthood and symptoms of PTSD are common in this patient group [
      • Fiszman A.
      • Alves-Leon S.V.
      • Nunes R.G.
      • D'Andrea I.
      • Figueira I.
      Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress disorder in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a critical review.
      ,
      • Gray C.
      • Calderbank A.
      • Adewusi J.
      • Hughes R.
      • Reuber M.
      Symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder in patients with functional neurological symptom disorder.
      ]. Shame has been identified as an important mediator between early experiences of trauma and subsequent PTSD [
      • López-Castro T.
      • Saraiya T.
      • Zumberg-Smith K.
      • Dambreville N.
      Association Between Shame and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: a Meta-Analysis.
      ]. Shame is also likely to be a key emotion in individuals whose PNES appear to be related to be related to experiences involving ‘moral injury’. Such injuries have been described in veterans with PNES in whom they are often associated with PTSD [
      • LaFrance Jr., W.C.
      • Vo P.
      • Baird G.
      • East R.
      • Stein N.R.
      Moral injury in Veterans with nonepileptic seizures.
      ]. They have been defined as the lasting psychological, biological, behavioral, spiritual, and social impact of “perpetrating, failing to prevent, bearing witness to, or learning about acts that transgress deeply held moral beliefs and expectations” [
      • Litz B.T.
      • Stein N.
      • Delaney E.
      • Lebowitz L.
      • Nash W.P.
      • Silva C.
      • Maguen S.
      Moral injury and moral repair in war veterans: a preliminary model and intervention strategy.
      ]. Furthermore, shame could play a role in the substantial subgroup of patients with PNES who deny a history of major trauma or significant neglect. In some of these patients, PNES disorders may be precipitated by life events which do not have the characteristics of major trauma, although they are traumatizing for the individual concerned because of their previous life experiences or particular circumstances [
      • Binzer M.
      • Stone J.
      • Sharpe M.
      Recent onset pseudoseizures–clues to aetiology.
      ,
      • Reuber M.
      • Howlett S.
      • Khan A.
      • Grünewald R.A.
      Non-epileptic seizures and other functional neurological symptoms: predisposing, precipitating, and perpetuating factors.
      ]. Some of these life events – for instance the loss of employment or illness leading to a significant change in status or self-perception – could be linked to PNES through intolerable levels of shame [
      • Bowman E.S.
      • Markand O.N.
      The contribution of life events to pseudoseizure occurrence in adults.
      ]. Shame may also be the key mediator between “unspeakable” secrets and forced choices with no good outcome, which one research group identified as an underlying dynamic leading to PNES in 13 of 14 families [
      • Griffith J.L.
      • Polles A.
      • Griffith M.E.
      Pseudoseizures, families, and unspeakable dilemmas.
      ].

      5.4 Emotion processing abnormalities associated with PNES

      As discussed above, early life experiences of trauma and neglect, as well as severe traumatic experiences in later life, are closely associated with the subsequent dysregulation of emotions. While PNES may arise in the context of over- or underregulated emotions [
      • Brown R.J.
      • Bouska J.F.
      • Frow A.
      • Kirkby A.
      • Baker G.A.
      • Kemp S.
      • Reuber M.
      Emotional dysregulation, alexithymia, and attachment in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
      ,
      • Reuber M.
      • Pukrop R.
      • Bauer J.
      • Derfuss R.
      • Elger C.E.
      Multidimensional assessment of personality in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures.
      ], and a broad range of abnormalities of emotion processing have been described in patients with PNES [
      • Williams I.A.
      • Levita L.
      • Reuber M.
      Emotion dysregulation in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a systematic review based on the extended process model.
      ], avoidance and alexithymia are most prominent [
      • Novakova B.
      • Howlett S.
      • Baker R.
      • Reuber M.
      Emotion processing and psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: a cross-sectional comparison of patients and healthy controls.
      ]. Avoidance could be considered universal for PNES if dissociation is interpreted as an acute avoidance mechanism. Excessive avoidance also characterizes the non-seizure thinking and behavior of many patients with PNES [
      • Dimaro L.V.
      • Dawson D.L.
      • Roberts N.A.
      • Brown I.
      • Moghaddam N.G.
      • Reuber M.
      Anxiety and avoidance in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: the role of implicit and explicit anxiety.
      ,
      • Bakvis P.
      • Spinhoven P.
      • Zitman F.G.
      • Roelofs K.
      Automatic avoidance tendencies in patients with Psychogenic Non Epileptic Seizures.
      ]. Alexithymia has been identified in over 90% of patients with PNES [
      • Bewley J.
      • Murphy P.N.
      • Mallows J.
      • Baker G.A.
      Does alexithymia differentiate between patients with nonepileptic seizures, patients with epilepsy, and nonpatient controls?.
      ]. These clinically important traits can arise as a consequence of the combination of trauma and shame. For instance, in individuals with eating disorders, shame has been shown to play a central role in the perception of an adverse self-image. Evidence suggests that, in this clinical context, alexithymia may result from previous unelaborated traumatic experiences and feelings of shame [
      • Franzoni E.
      • Gualandi S.
      • Caretti V.
      • Schimmenti A.
      • Di Pietro E.
      • Pellegrini G.
      • et al.
      The relationship between alexithymia, shame, trauma, and body image disorders: investigation over a large clinical sample.
      ]. Shame has also been identified as a mediator between current distress and alexithymia [
      • Rice S.M.
      • Kealy D.
      • Oliffe J.L.
      • Treeby M.S.
      • Ogrodniczuk J.S.
      Shame and guilt mediate the effects of alexithymia on distress and suicide-related behaviours among men.
      ]. In other words, it may explain why an important subgroup of patients with PNES have difficulties identifying or describing their emotions or acknowledging previous traumatic experiences [
      • Brown R.J.
      • Bouska J.F.
      • Frow A.
      • Kirkby A.
      • Baker G.A.
      • Kemp S.
      • et al.
      Emotional dysregulation, alexithymia, and attachment in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
      ].
      PNES in patients with a history of trauma could be triggered by acute shame with dissociation functioning as a shame avoidance mechanism. This mechanism could effectively suspend these patients in a state of alexithymia, to which they may already have been predisposed, by ‘protecting’ them from fully realizing their own distress, vulnerability or helplessness [
      • Dimaro L.V.
      • Roberts N.A.
      • Moghaddam N.G.
      • Dawson D.L.
      • Brown I.
      • Reuber M.
      Implicit and explicit self-esteem discrepancies in people with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
      ]. In the absence of being able to adequately label one's own emotional state, other “safe” interpretations of bodily sensations – such as heightened attention to somatic cues (which in turn may trigger or perpetuate PNES episodes; [
      • Fobian A.D.
      • Long D.M.
      • Szaflarski J.P.
      Retraining and control therapy for pediatric psychogenic non-epileptic seizures.
      ]) – may fill the gap. This interpretation of PNES could explain the phenomenon that, compared to patients with focal epileptic seizures involving anxiety, those with PNES are more likely to acknowledge ‘physical’ ictal arousal than ‘mental’ anxiety symptoms, an observation which has given rise to the interpretation of PNES as attacks of “panic without panic” [
      • Goldstein L.H.
      • Mellers J.D.
      Ictal symptoms of anxiety, avoidance behaviour, and dissociation in patients with dissociative seizures.
      ].
      Paradoxically, the dissociation associated with PNES may also disrupt the individual's ability to fully “access” and process shame experiences. Thus, it may require extra effort to do so, and, in some cases, an inability to do so without intervention and re-training. As studies with other clinical populations reveal (see Section 4.1), atypical neural processing of shame may result in its persistence or more consequential repercussions for emotional functioning.

      5.5 Comorbid mental disorders

      There is a complex relationship between PNES and “comorbid” mental disorders. Only about one third of patients with PNES do not meet DSM-4 diagnostic criteria for another current mental disorder [
      • Goldstein L.H.
      • Robinson E.J.
      • Reuber M.
      • Chalder T.
      • Callaghan H.
      • Eastwood C.
      • et al.
      Characteristics of 698 patients with dissociative seizures: a UK multicenter study.
      ]. While depression is the commonest mental disorder identified in patients with PNES, more specific links exist between PNES, anxiety disorders, PTSD and personality disorders [
      • Diprose W.
      • Sundram F.
      • Menkes D.B.
      Psychiatric comorbidity in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures compared with epilepsy.
      ], especially borderline personality disorder. In view of the role that increased chronic shame, shame-proneness and dysregulated shame may play in these disorders, shame could be a common mediator between the causes and manifestations of these mental illnesses and PNES. As such, shame could serve as a unifying etiological factor that helps to explain some of the clinical and psychopathological heterogeneity which characterizes PNES patient populations.

      5.6 PNES and stigma

      Over 90% of patients with PNES report being affected by stigma [
      • MacDuffie K.E.
      • Grubbs L.
      • Best T.
      • LaRoche S.
      • Mildon B.
      • Myers L.
      • Rommelfanger K.S.
      Stigma and functional neurological disorder: a research agenda targeting the clinical encounter.
      ], and those who feel stigmatized have a lower Health Related Quality of Life [
      • MacDuffie K.E.
      • Grubbs L.
      • Best T.
      • LaRoche S.
      • Mildon B.
      • Myers L.
      • Rommelfanger K.S.
      Stigma and functional neurological disorder: a research agenda targeting the clinical encounter.
      ,
      • Rawlings G.H.
      • Brown I.
      • Reuber M.
      Deconstructing stigma in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: an exploratory study.
      ]. Among those with PNES, Stigma is most likely perceived by those people with high levels of anxiety and low self-control or education [
      • Robson C.
      • Myers L.
      • Pretorius C.
      • Lian O.S.
      • Reuber M.
      Health related quality of life of people with non-epileptic seizures: the role of socio-demographic characteristics and stigma.
      ]. This stigma could be related to the lack of acceptability of mental disorders: many patients certainly experience the re-labeling of their seizure disorder from epilepsy – in itself a heavily stigmatized disorder – to a ‘psychiatric’ condition as stigmatizing [
      • Rawlings G.H.
      • Brown I.
      • Reuber M.
      Deconstructing stigma in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: an exploratory study.
      ,
      • Karterud H.N.
      • Knizek B.L.
      • Nakken K.O.
      Changing the diagnosis from epilepsy to PNES: patients' experiences and understanding of their new diagnosis.
      ]. However, there are also bidirectional links between stigma and shame. Chronic shame can arise as a result of societal stigmatization based on salient aspects of one's identity, e.g., gender, health status, disability, race, sexuality, weight or ethnicity. However, high shame proneness and the low levels of self-esteem associated with it are also likely to increase vulnerability to stigma [
      • Dimaro L.V.
      • Roberts N.A.
      • Moghaddam N.G.
      • Dawson D.L.
      • Brown I.
      • Reuber M.
      Implicit and explicit self-esteem discrepancies in people with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
      ]: We note that the process of stigmatization [as described by,
      • Link B.G.
      • Phelan J.C.
      Conceptualizing Stigma.
      ] has a lot in common with the experience of shame (cf guilt) - both involve the identification of a relevant difference from a norm that is taken to signify a flawed individual (in the case of stigma) or self (in the case of shame). Patients’ self-perception of a 'spoiled identity' may therefore make those with PNES particularly vulnerable to the perception of stigma and interact with (very real) enacted societal stigma to drive levels of shame up to a level at which it becomes debilitating or pathological and affect a person's life chances, relationships and health outcomes [
      • Dolezal L.
      • Lyons B.
      Health-related shame: an affective determinant of health?.
      ].

      5.7 Possible neurobiological links between PNES and shame

      While our understanding of the neurobiological processes underpinning PNES remains incomplete, there is considerable overlap between patterns of regional activation and functional connectivity implicated in PNES pathophysiology or shame experiences. To date, knowledge of PNES and pathophysiology more broadly in Functional Neurological Symptom Disorder (FND) implicates multiple neural circuits and networks, including those underpinning sensorimotor, attentional, emotional, and cognitive control processes (i.e., sensorimotor and TPJ; salience; limbic; dorsal and ventral attention; cognitive control and motor planning) [
      • Drane D.L.
      • Fani N.
      • Hallett M.
      • Khalsa S.S.
      • Perez D.L.
      • Roberts N.A.
      A framework for understanding the pathophysiology of functional neurological disorder.
      ,
      • Perez D.L.
      • Dworetzky B.A.
      • Dickerson B.C.
      • Leung L.
      • Cohn R.
      • Baslet G.
      • Silbersweig D.A.
      An integrative neurocircuit perspective on psychogenic nonepileptic seizures and functional movement disorders: neural functional unawareness.
      ]. Particular attention has been paid to the salience network [
      • Seeley W.W.
      • Menon V.
      • Schatzberg A.F.
      • Keller J.
      • Glover G.H.
      • Kenna H.
      • Greicius M.D.
      Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control.
      ], including the anterior and middle cingulate cortex, insula, amygdala, and periaqueductal gray [
      • Ospina J.P.
      • Jalilianhasanpour R.
      • Perez D.L.
      The role of the anterior and midcingulate cortex in the neurobiology of functional neurologic disorder.
      ]. Disruptions in these networks coincide with challenges appropriately attending to, prioritizing, and interpreting internal and external emotional/sensory information. These challenges may be magnified in shame, which is at the intersection of self- and other- evaluation because it involves self-reflection vis-à-vis social norms. Indeed, studies of functional connectivity in PNES (and functional movement disorders) point to alterations in connectivity among regions relevant to self-referential processing (TPJ, posterior cingulate cortex, precuneus) [
      • Perez D.L.
      • Dworetzky B.A.
      • Dickerson B.C.
      • Leung L.
      • Cohn R.
      • Baslet G.
      • Silbersweig D.A.
      An integrative neurocircuit perspective on psychogenic nonepileptic seizures and functional movement disorders: neural functional unawareness.
      ,
      • Sojka P.
      • Diez I.
      • Bareš M.
      • Perez D.L.
      Individual differences in interoceptive accuracy and prediction error in motor functional neurological disorders: a DTI study.
      ].
      Another possible account of the pathogenesis of PNES based on dysfunction in these networks and centres implicates poor interoception as a key factor [
      • Williams I.A.
      • Reuber M.
      • Levita L.
      Interoception and stress in patients with Functional Neurological Symptom Disorder.
      ]. Poor interoception, reflected in heightened attention to or misinterpretation of somatic cues [
      • Khalsa S.S.
      • Adolphs R.
      • Cameron O.G.
      • Critchley H.D.
      • Davenport P.W.
      • Feinstein J.S.
      • et al.
      Interoception and mental health: a roadmap.
      ], may be associated with generation of subsequent “prediction errors” [
      • Edwards M.J.
      • Adams R.A.
      • Brown H.
      • Pareés I.
      • Friston K.J.
      A Bayesian account of 'hysteria'.
      ], as well as a disrupted sense of self or self-agency (a sense of voluntary control over one's physical actions) [
      • Sojka P.
      • Diez I.
      • Bareš M.
      • Perez D.L.
      Individual differences in interoceptive accuracy and prediction error in motor functional neurological disorders: a DTI study.
      ]. Success in these latter domains requires appropriate recruitment of the anterior insula (sense of self) and TPJ (sense of self-agency), as is the case in shame and other self-conscious emotions (described earlier). Thus, we speculate that neurobiological vulnerabilities associated with disruptions in these other processes in PNES might coincide with disruptions in constructive experiencing and processing of self-conscious emotions also. For example, there is a lack of agency demonstrated in PNES and other FNDs [
      • Maurer C.W.
      • LaFaver K.
      • Ameli R.
      • Epstein S.A.
      • Hallett M.
      • Horovitz S.G.
      Impaired self-agency in functional movement disorders: a resting-state fMRI study.
      ,
      • Nahab F.B.
      • Kundu P.
      • Maurer C.
      • Shen Q.
      • Hallett M.
      Impaired sense of agency in functional movement disorders: an fMRI study.
      ] – both behaviorally and in terms of neurobiological correlates (e.g. decreased connectivity in the right TPJ). We can speculate that those with PNES may be less likely to experience guilt, which also is associated with TPJ activation and is a relatively agentic state, often motivating reparations or corrective action. Instead, when confronted with a troubling interpersonal situation, neurobiological vulnerabilities may lead individuals with PNES to appraisals and response tendencies favoring shame and withdrawal.

      6. Summary

      This narrative review has drawn attention to the potential importance of the social emotion of shame to PNES, a disorder characterized by an episodic disruption of normal levels of self-control and awareness, in which seizures are usually interpreted as a dissociative response to internal or external triggers associated with aversive emotions. Although shame arguably pervades much of the literature on the presentation and etiology of PNES, it has rarely been explicitly named in previous writings on PNES. In this review, we have demonstrated how well much of what we know about the early development of shame and the factors which determine normal or abnormal shame development (like the presence or absence of healthy attachments in early life, and the experience of trauma in early or later life) matches what we know about factors predisposing to the development of PNES. What is more, excessive levels of shame and reduced shame tolerance or increased shame avoidance may also represent important links between predisposing and precipitating or triggering factors of PNES disorders. We argue that a close association of PNES with shame may interact with enacted stigma (which is clearly experienced by patients around the world [
      • Robson C.
      • Lian O.S.
      “Blaming, shaming, humiliation”: stigmatising medical interactions among people with non-epileptic seizures [version 2; peer review: 2 approved, 1 approved with reservations].
      ] and thereby exacerbate disabilities directly related to seizure experiences.
      As has been suggested for PTSD, an aetiological model centrally incorporating shame may compliment previous fear avoidance models of PNES. In such an aetiological model for PNES, potential genetic factors co-determining shame sensitivity and tolerance interact with life experiences (especially attachment experiences and traumatic life events in early childhood) to create a level of vulnerability characterized by elevated shame proneness, low shame tolerance, and dysregulated shame processing. Shame-related problems are exacerbated by the public exhibition of emotion processing which patients may perceive their dissociative seizures to represent, and by a vicious circle involving the stigma associated with having a mental disorder and the shame fostered by the self-perception of a ‘spoiled identity’.
      These ideas are in keeping with the current understanding of the neurobiological underpinnings of shame and PNES: Mounting evidence for connections between limbic and motor control circuits may help explain triggers of PNES [
      • van der Kruijs S.J.
      • Bodde N.M.
      • Vaessen M.J.
      • Lazeron R.H.
      • Vonck K.
      • Boon P.
      • Jansen J.F.
      Functional connectivity of dissociation in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures.
      ]. Experiences of shame may be more central to these emotion-motor connections than previously recognized, given that shame is also associated with activation in motor-related areas (SMA, cerebellum). In short, models of PNES suggest emotion-triggered unfiltered/ungated motor output. Shame may be one of the most important emotions triggering PNES, for physiological reasons based on the regions involved in self-conscious emotion processing, as well as the theoretical clinical reasons outlined above. In other words, the “pain” of shame may be physically and neuropsychiatrically embodied.

      7. Limitations

      We fully acknowledge the limitations of this review and of our understanding of the relationship between shame and PNES. The subjectivity, lack of universally accepted definitions (of shame or PNES) or widely used validated measures makes emotions such as shame difficult to study or discuss. These difficulties are compounded by the fact that patients are likely to find it much harder to talk about shame than about other emotions and a dearth of research specifically focusing on the role of shame in those with PNES. In view of the lack of directly informative studies, most of our arguments rely on associations or indirect evidence linking PNES with shame. Pending further research we admit that, even if associations between PNES and shame exist, relationships may not be directly causal but implicate other factors such as comorbidities of PNES or shared risk factors for chronic shame or abnormal shame responses and PNES such as early childhood trauma and neglect.

      8. Conclusions

      Despite these limitations we think that there is a compelling case for researchers to explore chronic shame and shame dysregulation in patients with PNES. Even in the absence of further evidence, the well-recognized links between excessive shame and common comorbidities of PNES such as depression and PTSD and the well-characterized relationships between chronic shame and stigma suggest that clinicians providing care for patients with PNES need to be alert to the importance of shame in patients with this disorder. Interested readers are referred to a linked review to learn more about the role of shame in the treatment of PNES disorders [
      • Gray C.
      • Reuber M.
      • Roberts N.
      • Levita L.
      • Myers L.
      Shame in the treatment of patients with Psychogenic Nonepileptic Seizures: the elephant in the room?.
      ].

      Declaration of Competing Interest

      Markus Reuber: Payments from Elsevier as Editor-in-Chief of Seizure, educational grant from UCB Pharma. Markus Reuber and Lorna Myers: Income from book authorships (including books about PNES). None of these interests should have any bearing on the content of this article. The other authors declare that they have no known competing financial interests or personal relationships that could have appeared to influence the work reported in this paper.

      Acknowledgment

      The authors are grateful for the constructive comments received on their manuscript from Dr Alistair Wardrope.

      References

        • Levenson R.W.
        Human emotions: a functional view.
        The nature of emotion: fundamental questions. 1994; : 123-126
        • Barrett L.F.
        • Mesquita B.
        • Ochsner K.N.
        • Gross J.J.
        The experience of emotion.
        Ann Rev Psychol. 2006; 58: 373-403
        • Levenson R.W.
        • Soto J.
        • Pole N.
        Emotion, biology, and culture.
        Handbook of cultural psychology. 2007; (Editors)
        • Williams I.A.
        • Levita L.
        • Reuber M.
        Emotion dysregulation in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a systematic review based on the extended process model.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2018; 86: 37-48
        • Brown R.J.
        • Reuber M.
        Psychological and psychiatric aspects of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES): a systematic review.
        Clin Psychol Rev. 2016; 45: 157-182
        • Krámská L.
        • Hrešková L.
        • Vojtěch Z.
        • Krámský D.
        • Myers L.
        Maladaptive emotional regulation in patients diagnosed with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES) compared with healthy volunteers.
        Seizure. 2020; 78: 7-11
        • Pick S.
        • Goldstein L.H.
        • Perez D.L.
        • Nicholson T.R.
        Emotional processing in functional neurological disorder: a review, biopsychosocial model and research agenda.
        J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2019; 90: 704-711
        • Roberts N.A.
        • Burleson M.H.
        • Weber D.J.
        • Larson A.
        • Sergeant K.
        • Devine M.J.
        • Wang N.C.
        Emotion in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: responses to affective pictures.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2012; 24: 107-115
        • Rawlings G.H.
        • Jamnadas-Khoda J.
        • Broadhurst M.
        • Grünewald R.A.
        • Howell S.J.
        • Koepp M.
        • et al.
        Panic symptoms in transient loss of consciousness: frequency and diagnostic value in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures, epilepsy and syncope.
        Seizure. 2017; 48: 22-27
        • Dimaro L.V.
        • Dawson D.L.
        • Roberts N.A.
        • Brown I.
        • Moghaddam N.G.
        • Reuber M.
        Anxiety and avoidance in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: the role of implicit and explicit anxiety.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2014; 33C: 77-86
        • Griffith J.L.
        • Polles A.
        • Griffith M.E.
        Pseudoseizures, families, and unspeakable dilemmas.
        Psychosomatics. 1998; 39: 144-153
        • Hazard P.
        Freud's Teaching on Shame.
        Laval théologique et philosophique. 1969; 25: 234-267
        • López-Castro T.
        • Saraiya T.
        • Zumberg-Smith K.
        • Dambreville N.
        Association Between Shame and Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: a Meta-Analysis.
        J Trauma Stress. 2019; 32: 484-495
        • Robinaugh D.J.
        • McNally R.J.
        Autobiographical memory for shame or guilt provoking events: association with psychological symptoms.
        Behav Res Ther. 2010; 48: 646-652
        • Rüsch N.
        • Lieb K.
        • Göttler I.
        • Hermann C.
        • Schramm E.
        • Richter H.
        • Bohus M.
        Shame and implicit self-concept in women with borderline personality disorder.
        Am J Psychiatry. 2007; 164: 500-508
        • Peters J.R.
        • Geiger P.J.
        Borderline personality disorder and self-conscious affect: too much shame but not enough guilt?.
        Personal Disorder. 2016; 7: 303-308
        • Hennig-Fast K.
        • Michl P.
        • Müller J.
        • Niedermeier N.
        • Coates U.
        • Müller N.
        • Meindl T.
        Obsessive-compulsive disorder–A question of conscience? An fMRI study of behavioural and neurofunctional correlates of shame and guilt.
        J Psychiatr Res. 2015; 68: 354-362
        • Weingarden H.
        • Renshaw K.D.
        Shame in the obsessive compulsive related disorders: a conceptual review.
        J Affect Disord. 2015; 171: 74-84
        • Kim S.
        • Thibodeau R.
        • Jorgensen R.S.
        Shame, guilt, and depressive symptoms: a meta-analytic review.
        Psychol Bull. 2011; 137: 68-96
        • Brown R.J.
        • Reuber M.
        Towards an integrative theory of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures (PNES).
        Clin Psychol Rev. 2016; 47: 55-70
        • Reuber M.
        The etiology of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: toward a biopsychosocial model.
        Neurol Clin. 2009; 27: 909-924
        • Cowen A.S.
        • Keltner D.
        Self-report captures 27 distinct categories of emotion bridged by continuous gradients.
        Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A,. 2017; 114: E7900-E7909
        • Tracy J.L.
        • Robins R.W.
        Putting the self into self-conscious emotions: a theoretical model.
        Psychological Inquiry. 2004; 15: 103-125
        • Tangney J.P.
        • Stuewig J.
        • Mashek D.J.
        Moral emotions and moral behavior.
        Ann Rev Psychol. 2007; 58: 345-372
        • Leach C.W.
        Understanding shame and guilt.
        Handbook of the psychology of self-forgiveness. Springer International Publishing AG, Cham, Switzerland2017: 17-28
        • Ferguson T.J.
        • Crowley S.L.
        Measure for measure: a multitrait-multimethod analysis of guilt and shame.
        J Personal Assess. 1997; 69: 425-441
        • Eisenberg N.
        Emotion, regulation, and moral development.
        Annu Rev Psychol. 2000; 51: 665-697
        • Cândea D.M.
        • Szentagotai-Tătar A.
        Shame-proneness, guilt-proneness and anxiety symptoms: a meta-analysis.
        J Anxiety Disord. 2018; 58: 78-106
        • Mills R.S.L.
        • Hastings P.D.
        • Serbin L.A.
        • Stack D.M.
        • Abela J.R.Z.
        • Arbeau K.A.
        • et al.
        Depressogenic thinking and shame proneness in the development of internalizing problems.
        Child Psychiatry Human Dev. 2015; 46: 194-208
        • Luby J.
        • Belden A.
        • Sullivan J.
        • Hayen R.
        • McCadney A.
        • Spitznagel E.
        Shame and guilt in preschool depression: evidence for elevations in self-conscious emotions in depression as early as age 3.
        J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2009; 50: 1156-1166
        • Muris P.
        • Meesters C.
        Small or big in the eyes of the other: on the developmental psychopathology of self-conscious emotions as shame, guilt, and pride.
        Clin Child Family Psychol Rev. 2014; 17: 19-40
        • De Rubeis S.
        • Hollenstein T.
        Individual differences in shame and depressive symptoms during early adolescence.
        Personal Individ Differ. 2009; 46: 477-482
        • Stuewig J.
        • Tangney J.P.
        • Kendall S.
        • Folk J.B.
        • Meyer C.R.
        • Dearing R.L.
        Children’s proneness to shame and guilt predict risky and illegal behaviors in young adulthood.
        Child Psychiatry Hum Dev. 2015; 46: 217-227
        • Tilghman-Osborne C.
        • Cole D.A.
        • Felton J.W.
        • Ciesla J.A.
        Relation of guilt, shame, behavioral and characterological self-blame to depressive symptoms in adolescents over time.
        J Soc Clin Psychol. 2008; 27: 809-842
        • Fergus T.A.
        • Valentiner D.P.
        • McGrath P.B.
        • Jencius S.
        Shame- and guilt-proneness: relationships with anxiety disorder symptoms in a clinical sample.
        J Anxiety Disord. 2010; 24: 811-815
        • Kim S.
        • Thibodeau R.
        • Jorgensen R.S.
        Shame, guilt, and depressive symptoms: a meta-analytic review.
        Psychol Bull. 2011; 137: 68-96
        • Shahar G.
        Personality, shame, and the breakdown of social bonds: the voice of quantitative depression research.
        Psychiatry. 2001; 64: 228-239
        • Treeby M.
        • Bruno R.
        Shame and guilt-proneness: divergent implications for problematic alcohol use and drinking to cope with anxiety and depression symptomatology.
        Personal Individ Differ. 2012; 53: 613-617
        • Woien S.
        • Ernst H.
        • Patock-Peckham J.
        • Nagoshi C.
        Validation of the TOSCA to measure shame and guilt.
        Personal Individ Differ. 2003; 35: 313-326
        • Leach C.W.
        • Cidam A.
        When is shame linked to constructive approach orientation? A meta-analysis.
        J Pers Soc Psychol. 2015; 109: 983-1002
        • Wang L.
        • Pan H.
        • Zhang H.
        The effect of emotional intensity of shame on children’s prosocial behaviour.
        Eur J Dev Psychol. 2020; 17: 263-274
      1. Gaspar, A. and M. Henriques, Driven by shame: how a negative emotion may lead to prosocial behaviour. In: Sara Graca da Silva (ed): New interdisciplinary landscapes in morality and emotion. London: Routledge, chapter 4, 2018.

        • de Hooge I.E.
        • Breugelmans S.M.
        • Wagemans F.M.A.
        • Zeelenberg M.
        The social side of shame: approach versus withdrawal.
        Cogn Emot. 2018; 32: 1671-1677
        • Gross J.J.
        • Thompson R.A.
        Emotion regulation: conceptual foundations.
        Handbook of emotion regulation. The Guilford Press, New York, NY, US2007: 3-24
        • Dixon-Gordon K.L.
        • Aldao A.
        • De Los Reyes A.
        Emotion regulation in context: examining the spontaneous use of strategies across emotional intensity and type of emotion.
        Personal Individ Differ. 2015; 86: 271-276
        • Tangney J.P.
        • Wagner P.
        • Gramzow R.
        Proneness to shame, proneness to guilt, and psychopathology.
        J Abnorm Psychol. 1992; 101: 469-478
        • Schoenleber M.
        • Berenbaum H.
        Shame regulation in personality pathology.
        J Abnorm Psychol. 2012; 121: 433-446
        • Reuber M.
        • Pukrop R.
        • Bauer J.
        • Derfuss R.
        • Elger C.E.
        Multidimensional assessment of personality in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures.
        J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004; 75: 743
        • Beghi M.
        • Negrini P.B.
        • Perin C.
        • Peroni F.
        • Magaudda A.
        • Cerri C.
        • et al.
        Psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: so-called psychiatric comorbidity and underlying defense mechanisms.
        Neuropsychiatr Dis Treat. 2015; 11: 2519-2527
        • Mills R.S.L.
        Taking stock of the developmental literature on shame.
        Dev Rev. 2005; 25: 26-63
        • Barrett K.C.
        • Zahn-waxler C.
        • Cole P.M.
        Avoiders vs. Amenders: implications for the investigation of guilt and shame during Toddlerhood?.
        Cogn Emot. 1993; 7: 481-505
        • Cole P.M.
        • Barrett K.C.
        • Zahn-Waxler C.
        Emotion displays in two-year-olds during mishaps.
        Child Dev. 1992; 63: 314-324
        • Kochanska G.
        • DeVet K.
        • Goldman M.
        • Murray K.
        • Putnam S.P.
        Maternal reports of conscience development and temperament in young children.
        Child Dev. 1994; 65: 852-868
        • Lewis M.
        Self-conscious emotions: embarrassment, pride,shame, and guilt.
        in: Lewis M. Haviland-Jones J.M. Handbook of emotions. Guilford Press, New York2000: 623-636 (Editors)
        • Barrett K.C.
        A functionalist perspective to the development of emotions.
        in: Mascolo M.F. Griffin S. What develops in emotional development? Springer, US: Boston, MA1998: 109-133
        • Eisenberg N.
        • Fabes R.A.
        • Guthrie I.K.
        • Reiser M.
        Dispositional emotionality and regulation: their role in predicting quality of social functioning.
        J Pers Soc Psychol. 2000; 78: 136-157
        • Kochanska G.
        • Gross J.N.
        • Lin M.H.
        • Nichols K.E.
        Guilt in young children: development, determinants, and relations with a broader system of standards.
        Child Dev. 2002; 73: 461-482
        • Denham S.
        • Bassett H.
        • Wyatt T.
        The socialization of emotional competence.
        Handbook of socialization: theory and research. 2007;
        • Eisenberg N.
        • Fabes R.A.
        • Spinrad T.L.
        Handbook of child psychology: volume 3.Social, emotional, and personality development.
        Wiley, New York2006: 646-718
        • Lewis M.
        Shame: the exposed self.
        Shame: the exposed self. Free Press. xii, New York, NY, US1992: 275 (275-xii)
        • Berti A.
        • Garattoni C.
        • Venturini B.
        The Understanding of sadness, guilt, and shame in 5-, 7-, and 9-year-old children.
        Genet Soc Gener Psychol Monogr. 2000; 126: 293-318
        • Orth U.
        • Robins R.W.
        • Soto C.J.
        Tracking the trajectory of shame, guilt, and pride across the life span.
        J Pers Soc Psychol. 2010; 99: 1061-1071
        • Wong Y.
        • Tsai J.
        Cultural models of shame and guilt.
        The self-conscious emotions: theory and research. Guilford Press, New York, NY, US2007: 209-223
        • Szentágotai-Tătar A.
        • Chiș A.
        • Vulturar R.
        • Dobrean A.
        • Cândea D.M.
        • Miu A.C.
        Shame and guilt-proneness in adolescents: gene-environment interactions.
        PLOS ONE. 2015; 10e0134716
        • Muris P.
        • Meesters C.
        • Heijmans J.
        • van Hulten S.
        • Kaanen L.
        • Oerlemans B.
        • Tielemans T.
        Lack of guilt, guilt, and shame: a multi-informant study on the relations between self-conscious emotions and psychopathology in clinically referred children and adolescents.
        Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2016; 25: 383-396
        • Else-Quest N.M.
        • Higgins A.
        • Allison C.
        • Morton L.C.
        Gender differences in self-conscious emotional experience: a meta-analysis.
        Psychol Bull. 2012; 138: 947-981
        • Claesson K.
        • Sohlberg S.
        Internalized shame and early interactions characterized by indifference, abandonment and rejection: replicated findings.
        Clin Psychol Psychother. 2002; 9: 277-284
        • Gilbert P.
        • Allan S.
        • Goss K.
        Parental representations, shame, interpersonal problems, and vulnerability to psychopathology.
        Clin Psychol Psychother. 1996; 3: 23-34
        • Lutwak N.
        • Ferrari J.
        Understanding shame in adults: retrospective perceptions of parental-bonding during childhood.
        J Nerv Ment Dis. 1997; 185: 595-598
        • Kelley S.A.
        • Brownell C.A.
        • Campbell S.B.
        Mastery motivation and self-evaluative affect in toddlers: longitudinal relations with maternal behavior.
        Child Dev. 2000; 71: 1061-1071
        • Han D.H.
        • Kim S.M.
        • Lee Y.S.
        • Renshaw P.F.
        The effect of family therapy on the changes in the severity of on-line game play and brain activity in adolescents with on-line game addiction.
        Psychiatry Res. 2012; 202: 126-131
        • Stuewig J.
        • McCloskey L.A.
        The relation of child maltreatment to shame and guilt among adolescents: psychological routes to depression and delinquency.
        Child Maltreat. 2005; 10: 324-336
        • Mills R.S.L.
        Possible antecedents and developmental implications of shame in young girls.
        Infant Child Dev. 2003; 12: 329-349
        • Mills R.S.L.
        • Arbeau K.A.
        • Lall D.I.K.
        • De Jaeger A.E.
        Parenting and child characteristics in the prediction of shame in early and middle childhood.
        Merrill-Palmer Q. 2010; 56: 500-528
        • Parisette-Sparks A.
        • Bufferd S.J.
        • Klein D.N.
        Parental predictors of children's shame and guilt at age 6 in a multimethod, longitudinal study.
        J Clin Child Adolesc Psychol. 2017; 46: 721-731
        • Salmon P.
        • Al-Marzooqi S.M.
        • Baker G.
        • Reilly J.
        Childhood family dysfunction and associated abuse in patients with nonepileptic seizures: towards a causal model.
        Psychosom Med. 2003; 65: 695-700
        • Dong M.
        • Anda R.F.
        • Dube S.R.
        • Giles W.H.
        • Felitti V.J.
        The relationship of exposure to childhood sexual abuse to other forms of abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction during childhood.
        Child Abuse Neglect. 2003; 27: 625-639
        • Dong M.
        • Anda R.F.
        • Felitti V.J.
        • Dube S.R.
        • Williamson D.F.
        • Thompson T.J.
        • et al.
        The interrelatedness of multiple forms of childhood abuse, neglect, and household dysfunction.
        Child Abuse Neglect. 2004; 28: 771-784
        • Dobson K.S.
        • McLarnon M.J.W.
        • Pandya K.
        • Pusch D.
        A latent profile analysis of adverse childhood experiences and adult health in a community sample.
        Child Abuse Neglect. 2021; 114104927
        • Sanchis-Sanchis A.
        • Grau M.D.
        • Moliner A.-.R.
        • Morales-Murillo C.P.
        Effects of age and gender in emotion regulation of children and adolescents.
        Front Psychol. 2020; : 11
        • Cole P.M.
        Moving ahead in the study of the development of emotion regulation.
        Int J Behav Dev. 2014; 38: 203-207
        • Szentágotai-Tătar A.
        • Miu A.C.
        Individual differences in emotion regulation, childhood trauma and proneness to shame and guilt in adolescence.
        PLoS One. 2016; 11e0167299
        • Saraiya T.
        • Lopez-Castro T.
        Ashamed and afraid: a scoping review of the role of shame in Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder (PTSD).
        J Clin Med. 2016; 5: 94
        • Weingarden H.
        • Renshaw K.D.
        Shame in the obsessive compulsive related disorders: a conceptual review.
        J Affect Disord. 2015; 171: 74-84
        • Zahn R.
        • Lythe K.E.
        • Gethin J.A.
        • Green S.
        • Deakin J.F.W.
        • Young A.H.
        • et al.
        The role of self-blame and worthlessness in the psychopathology of major depressive disorder.
        J Affect Disord. 2015; 186: 337-341
        • Pulcu E.
        • Lythe K.
        • Elliott R.
        • Green S.
        • Moll J.
        • Deakin J.F.
        • Zahn R.
        Increased amygdala response to shame in remitted major depressive disorder.
        PLoS One. 2014; 9: e86900
        • Whitehead K.
        • Kandler R.
        • Reuber M.
        Patients' and neurologists' perception of epilepsy and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
        Epilepsia. 2013; 54: 708-717
        • Stone J.
        • Binzer M.
        • Sharpe M.
        Illness beliefs and locus of control: a comparison of patients with pseudoseizures and epilepsy.
        J Psychosom Res. 2004; 57: 541-547
        • Finger E.C.
        • Marsh A.A.
        • Kamel N.
        • Mitchell D.G.
        • Blair J.R.
        Caught in the act: the impact of audience on the neural response to morally and socially inappropriate behavior.
        Neuroimage. 2006; 33: 414-421
        • Wagner U.
        • N'Diaye K.
        • Ethofer T.
        • Vuilleumier P.
        Guilt-specific processing in the prefrontal cortex.
        Cereb Cortex. 2011; 21: 2461-2470
        • Lindquist K.A.
        • Wager T.D.
        • Kober H.
        • Bliss-Moreau E.
        • Barrett L.F.
        The brain basis of emotion: a meta-analytic review.
        Behav Brain Sci. 2012; 35: 121-143
        • Roth L.
        • Kaffenberger T.
        • Herwig U.
        • Brühl A.B.
        Brain activation associated with pride and shame.
        Neuropsychobiology. 2014; 69: 95-106
        • Bastin C.
        • Harrison B.J.
        • Davey C.G.
        • Moll J.
        • Whittle S.
        Feelings of shame, embarrassment and guilt and their neural correlates: a systematic review.
        Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2016; 71: 455-471
        • Michl P.
        • Meindl T.
        • Meister F.
        • Born C.
        • Engel R.R.
        • Reiser M.
        • et al.
        Neurobiological underpinnings of shame and guilt: a pilot fMRI study.
        Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci. 2014; 9: 150-157
        • Zhu R.
        • Feng C.
        • Zhang S.
        • Mai X.
        • Liu C.
        Differentiating guilt and shame in an interpersonal context with univariate activation and multivariate pattern analyses.
        Neuroimage. 2019; 186: 476-486
        • Zito G.A.
        • Wiest R.
        • Aybek S.
        Neural correlates of sense of agency in motor control: a neuroimaging meta-analysis.
        PLOS ONE. 2020; 15e0234321
        • Craig A.D.
        How do you feel — Now? The anterior insula and human awareness.
        Nat Rev Neurosci. 2009; 10: 59-70
        • Craig A.D.
        Significance of the insula for the evolution of human awareness of feelings from the body.
        Ann N Y Acad Sci. 2011; 1225: 72-82
        • Damasio A.
        • Carvalho G.B.
        The nature of feelings: evolutionary and neurobiological origins.
        Nat Rev Neurosci. 2013; 14: 143-152
        • Pulcu E.
        • Lythe K.
        • Elliott R.
        • Green S.
        • Moll J.
        • Deakin J.F.W.
        • Zahn R.
        Increased amygdala response to shame in remitted major depressive disorder.
        PLOS ONE. 2014; 9: e86900
        • Frewen P.A.
        • Dozois D.J.A.
        • Neufeld R.W.J.
        • Densmore M.
        • Stevens T.K.
        • Lanius R.A.
        Self-referential processing in women with PTSD: affective and neural response.
        Psychol Trauma. 2011; 3: 318-328
        • Balzarotti S.
        • Biassoni F.
        • Colombo B.
        • Ciceri M.R.
        Cardiac vagal control as a marker of emotion regulation in healthy adults: a review.
        Biol Psychol. 2017; 130: 54-66
        • Muhtadie L.
        • Koslov K.
        • Akinola M.
        • Mendes W.B.
        Vagal flexibility: a physiological predictor of social sensitivity.
        J Pers Soc Psychol. 2015; 109: 106-120
        • Freed S.
        • D’Andrea W.
        Autonomic arousal and emotion in victims of interpersonal violence: shame proneness but not anxiety predicts vagal tone.
        J Trauma Dissociation. 2015; 16: 367-383
        • Dickerson S.S.
        • Gruenewald T.L.
        • Kemeny M.E.
        When the social self is threatened: shame, physiology, and health.
        J Pers. 2004; 72: 1191-1216
        • Reuber M.
        The etiology of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: toward a biopsychosocial model.
        Neurol Clin. 2009; 27: 909-924
        • Keltner D.
        • Buswell B.N.
        Evidence for the distinctness of embarrassment, shame, and guilt: a study of recalled antecedents and facial expressions of emotion.
        Cogn Emot. 1996; 10: 155-171
        • Keltner D.
        Signs of appeasement: evidence for the distinct displays of embarrassment, amusement, and shame.
        American Psychological Association: US, 1995: 441-454
        • Izard C.E.
        Human emotions. emotions, personality, and psychotherapy.
        Plenum Press, New York1977
        • Widen S.C.
        • Christy A.M.
        • Hewett K.
        • Russell J.A.
        Do proposed facial expressions of contempt, shame, embarrassment, and compassion communicate the predicted emotion?.
        Cogn Emot. 2011; 25: 898-906
        • Keltner D.
        Evidence for the distinctness of embarrassment, shame, and guilt: a study of recalled antecedents and facial expressions of emotion.
        Cogn Emot. 1996; 10: 155-172
        • Hubsch C.
        • Baumann C.
        • Hingray C.
        • Gospodaru N.
        • Vignal J.P.
        • Vespignani H.
        • Maillard L.
        Clinical classification of psychogenic non-epileptic seizures based on video-EEG analysis and automatic clustering.
        J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2011; 82: 955-960
        • Bell W.L.
        • Park Y.D.
        • Thompson E.A.
        • Radtke R.A.
        Ictal cognitive assessment of partial seizures and pseudoseizures.
        Arch Neurol. 1998; 55: 1456-1459
        • Mökleby K.
        • Blomhoff S.
        • Malt U.F.
        • Dahlström A.
        • Tauböll E.
        • Gjerstad L.
        Psychiatric comorbidity and hostility in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures compared with somatoform disorders and healthy controls.
        Epilepsia. 2002; 43: 193-198
        • Ponnusamy A.
        • Marques J.L.B.
        • Reuber M.
        Comparison of heart rate variability parameters during complex partial seizures and psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
        Epilepsia. 2012; 53: 1314-1321
        • Freed S.
        • D’Andrea W.
        Autonomic arousal and emotion in victims of interpersonal violence: shame proneness but not anxiety predicts vagal tone.
        J Trauma Dissociation. 2015; 16: 367-383
        • Ponnusamy A.
        • Marques J.L.
        • Reuber M.
        Heart rate variability measures as biomarkers in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: potential and limitations.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2011; 22: 685-691
        • Bakvis P.
        • Spinhoven P.
        • Giltay E.J.
        • Kuyk J.
        • Edelbroek P.M.
        • Zitman F.G.
        • Roelofs K.
        Basal hypercortisolism and trauma in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
        Epilepsia. 2010; 51: 752-759
        • Roberts N.A.
        • Burleson M.H.
        • Torres D.L.
        • Parkhurst D.K.
        • Garrett R.
        • Mitchell L.B.
        • Wang N.C.
        Emotional reactivity as a vulnerability for psychogenic nonepileptic seizures? Responses while reliving specific emotions.
        J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2020; 32: 95-100
        • Romigi A.
        • Ricciardo Rizzo G.
        • Izzi F.
        • Guerrisi M.
        • Caccamo M.
        • Testa F.
        • et al.
        Heart rate variability parameters during psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: comparison between patients with pure PNES and comorbid epilepsy.
        Front Neurol. 2020; 11: 713
        • van der Kruijs S.J.M.
        • Vonck K.E.J.
        • Langereis G.R.
        • Feijs L.M.G.
        • Bodde N.M.G.
        • Lazeron R.H.C.
        • et al.
        Autonomic nervous system functioning associated with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: analysis of heart rate variability.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2016; 54: 14-19
        • van der Kruijs S.J.
        • Vonck K.E.
        • Langereis G.R.
        • Feijs L.M.
        • Bodde N.M.
        • Lazeron R.H.
        • et al.
        Autonomic nervous system functioning associated with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: analysis of heart rate variability.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2016; 54: 14-19
        • Baird G.L.
        • Harlow L.L.
        • Machan J.T.
        • Thomas D.
        • LaFrance Jr., W.C.
        Identifying seizure clusters in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2017; 73: 142-147
        • Kemp S.
        • Graham C.D.
        • Chan R.
        • Kitchingman H.
        • Vickerman K.
        • Reuber M.
        The frequency and management of seizures during psychological treatment among patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures and epilepsy.
        Epilepsia. 2018; 59: 844-853
        • Myers L.
        • Zandberg L.
        Prolonged exposure therapy for comorbid psychogenic nonepileptic seizures and posttraumatic stress disorder.
        Clinical Case Studies. 2017; 17: 3-20
        • Reuber M.
        • Micoulaud-Franchi J.A.
        • Gülich E.
        • Bartolomei F.
        • McGonigal A.
        Comment ce que disent les patients peut nous renseigner sur leurs crises non épileptiques psychogènes [What the patient’s history tells us about their nonepileptic seizures].
        Neurophysiol Clin. 2014; 44: 375-388
        • Ludwig L.
        • Pasman J.A.
        • Nicholson T.
        • Aybek S.
        • David A.S.
        • Tuck S.
        • Stone J.
        Stressful life events and maltreatment in conversion (functional neurological) disorder: systematic review and meta-analysis of case-control studies.
        Lancet Psychiatry. 2018; 5: 307-320
        • Sharpe D.
        • Faye C.
        Non-epileptic seizures and child sexual abuse: a critical review of the literature.
        Clin Psychol Rev. 2006; 26: 1020-1040
        • Holman N.
        • Kirkby A.
        • Duncan S.
        • Brown R.J.
        Adult attachment style and childhood interpersonal trauma in non-epileptic attack disorder.
        Epilepsy Res. 2008; 79: 84-89
        • Levita L.
        • Mayberry E.
        • Mehmood A.
        • Reuber M.
        Evaluation of LiNES: a new measure of trauma, negative affect, and relationship insecurity over the life span in persons with FND.
        J Neuropsychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2020; 32: 43-49
        • Keynejad R.C.
        • Frodl T.
        • Kanaan R.
        • Pariante C.
        • Reuber M.
        • Nicholson T.R.
        Stress and functional neurological disorders: mechanistic insights.
        J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2019; 90: 813-821
        • Fiszman A.
        • Alves-Leon S.V.
        • Nunes R.G.
        • D'Andrea I.
        • Figueira I.
        Traumatic events and posttraumatic stress disorder in patients with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: a critical review.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2004; 5: 818-825
        • Gray C.
        • Calderbank A.
        • Adewusi J.
        • Hughes R.
        • Reuber M.
        Symptoms of posttraumatic stress disorder in patients with functional neurological symptom disorder.
        Journal of Psychosomatic Res. 2020; 129109907
        • LaFrance Jr., W.C.
        • Vo P.
        • Baird G.
        • East R.
        • Stein N.R.
        Moral injury in Veterans with nonepileptic seizures.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2020; 102106681
        • Litz B.T.
        • Stein N.
        • Delaney E.
        • Lebowitz L.
        • Nash W.P.
        • Silva C.
        • Maguen S.
        Moral injury and moral repair in war veterans: a preliminary model and intervention strategy.
        Clin Psychol Rev. 2009; 29: 695-706
        • Binzer M.
        • Stone J.
        • Sharpe M.
        Recent onset pseudoseizures–clues to aetiology.
        Seizure. 2004; 13: 146-155
        • Reuber M.
        • Howlett S.
        • Khan A.
        • Grünewald R.A.
        Non-epileptic seizures and other functional neurological symptoms: predisposing, precipitating, and perpetuating factors.
        Psychosomatics. 2007; 48: 230-238
        • Bowman E.S.
        • Markand O.N.
        The contribution of life events to pseudoseizure occurrence in adults.
        Bull Menninger Clin. 1999; 63: 70-88
        • Brown R.J.
        • Bouska J.F.
        • Frow A.
        • Kirkby A.
        • Baker G.A.
        • Kemp S.
        • Reuber M.
        Emotional dysregulation, alexithymia, and attachment in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2013; 29: 178-183
        • Reuber M.
        • Pukrop R.
        • Bauer J.
        • Derfuss R.
        • Elger C.E.
        Multidimensional assessment of personality in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures.
        J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2004; 75: 743-748
        • Novakova B.
        • Howlett S.
        • Baker R.
        • Reuber M.
        Emotion processing and psychogenic non-epileptic seizures: a cross-sectional comparison of patients and healthy controls.
        Seizure. 2015; 29: 4-10
        • Bakvis P.
        • Spinhoven P.
        • Zitman F.G.
        • Roelofs K.
        Automatic avoidance tendencies in patients with Psychogenic Non Epileptic Seizures.
        Seizure-Eur J Epilepsy. 2011; 20: 628-634
        • Bewley J.
        • Murphy P.N.
        • Mallows J.
        • Baker G.A.
        Does alexithymia differentiate between patients with nonepileptic seizures, patients with epilepsy, and nonpatient controls?.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2005; 7: 430-437
        • Franzoni E.
        • Gualandi S.
        • Caretti V.
        • Schimmenti A.
        • Di Pietro E.
        • Pellegrini G.
        • et al.
        The relationship between alexithymia, shame, trauma, and body image disorders: investigation over a large clinical sample.
        Neuropsychiatric Dis Treat. 2013; 9: 185-193
        • Rice S.M.
        • Kealy D.
        • Oliffe J.L.
        • Treeby M.S.
        • Ogrodniczuk J.S.
        Shame and guilt mediate the effects of alexithymia on distress and suicide-related behaviours among men.
        Psychol Health Med. 2020; 25: 17-24
        • Brown R.J.
        • Bouska J.F.
        • Frow A.
        • Kirkby A.
        • Baker G.A.
        • Kemp S.
        • et al.
        Emotional dysregulation, alexithymia, and attachment in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2013; 29: 178-183
        • Dimaro L.V.
        • Roberts N.A.
        • Moghaddam N.G.
        • Dawson D.L.
        • Brown I.
        • Reuber M.
        Implicit and explicit self-esteem discrepancies in people with psychogenic nonepileptic seizures.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2015; 46: 109-117
        • Fobian A.D.
        • Long D.M.
        • Szaflarski J.P.
        Retraining and control therapy for pediatric psychogenic non-epileptic seizures.
        Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2020; 7: 1410-1419
        • Goldstein L.H.
        • Mellers J.D.
        Ictal symptoms of anxiety, avoidance behaviour, and dissociation in patients with dissociative seizures.
        J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006; 77: 616-621
        • Goldstein L.H.
        • Robinson E.J.
        • Reuber M.
        • Chalder T.
        • Callaghan H.
        • Eastwood C.
        • et al.
        Characteristics of 698 patients with dissociative seizures: a UK multicenter study.
        Epilepsia. 2019; 60: 2182-2193
        • Diprose W.
        • Sundram F.
        • Menkes D.B.
        Psychiatric comorbidity in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures compared with epilepsy.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2016; 56: 123-130
        • MacDuffie K.E.
        • Grubbs L.
        • Best T.
        • LaRoche S.
        • Mildon B.
        • Myers L.
        • Rommelfanger K.S.
        Stigma and functional neurological disorder: a research agenda targeting the clinical encounter.
        CNS Spectr. 2020; : 1-6
        • Rawlings G.H.
        • Brown I.
        • Reuber M.
        Deconstructing stigma in psychogenic nonepileptic seizures: an exploratory study.
        Epilepsy Behav. 2017; 74: 167-172
        • Robson C.
        • Myers L.
        • Pretorius C.
        • Lian O.S.
        • Reuber M.
        Health related quality of life of people with non-epileptic seizures: the role of socio-demographic characteristics and stigma.
        Seizure. 2018; 55: 93-99
        • Karterud H.N.
        • Knizek B.L.
        • Nakken K.O.
        Changing the diagnosis from epilepsy to PNES: patients' experiences and understanding of their new diagnosis.
        Seizure. 2010; 19: 40-46
        • Link B.G.
        • Phelan J.C.
        Conceptualizing Stigma.
        Ann Rev Sociol. 2001; 27: 363-385
        • Dolezal L.
        • Lyons B.
        Health-related shame: an affective determinant of health?.
        Med Humanit. 2017; 43: 257-263
        • Drane D.L.
        • Fani N.
        • Hallett M.
        • Khalsa S.S.
        • Perez D.L.
        • Roberts N.A.
        A framework for understanding the pathophysiology of functional neurological disorder.
        CNS Spectr. 2020; : 1-7
        • Perez D.L.
        • Dworetzky B.A.
        • Dickerson B.C.
        • Leung L.
        • Cohn R.
        • Baslet G.
        • Silbersweig D.A.
        An integrative neurocircuit perspective on psychogenic nonepileptic seizures and functional movement disorders: neural functional unawareness.
        Clin EEG Neurosci. 2015; 46: 4-15
        • Seeley W.W.
        • Menon V.
        • Schatzberg A.F.
        • Keller J.
        • Glover G.H.
        • Kenna H.
        • Greicius M.D.
        Dissociable intrinsic connectivity networks for salience processing and executive control.
        J Neurosci. 2007; 27: 2349-2356
        • Ospina J.P.
        • Jalilianhasanpour R.
        • Perez D.L.
        The role of the anterior and midcingulate cortex in the neurobiology of functional neurologic disorder.
        Handb Clin Neurol. 2019; 166: 267-279
        • Sojka P.
        • Diez I.
        • Bareš M.
        • Perez D.L.
        Individual differences in interoceptive accuracy and prediction error in motor functional neurological disorders: a DTI study.
        Human Brain Mapp. 2021; 42: 1434-1445
        • Williams I.A.
        • Reuber M.
        • Levita L.
        Interoception and stress in patients with Functional Neurological Symptom Disorder.
        Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2021; 26: 75-94
        • Khalsa S.S.
        • Adolphs R.
        • Cameron O.G.
        • Critchley H.D.
        • Davenport P.W.
        • Feinstein J.S.
        • et al.
        Interoception and mental health: a roadmap.
        Biolog Psychiatry. 2018; 3: 501-513
        • Edwards M.J.
        • Adams R.A.
        • Brown H.
        • Pareés I.
        • Friston K.J.
        A Bayesian account of 'hysteria'.
        Brain. 2012; 135: 3495-3512
        • Maurer C.W.
        • LaFaver K.
        • Ameli R.
        • Epstein S.A.
        • Hallett M.
        • Horovitz S.G.
        Impaired self-agency in functional movement disorders: a resting-state fMRI study.
        Neurology. 2016; 87: 564-570
        • Nahab F.B.
        • Kundu P.
        • Maurer C.
        • Shen Q.
        • Hallett M.
        Impaired sense of agency in functional movement disorders: an fMRI study.
        PLOS ONE. 2017; 12e0172502
        • Robson C.
        • Lian O.S.
        “Blaming, shaming, humiliation”: stigmatising medical interactions among people with non-epileptic seizures [version 2; peer review: 2 approved, 1 approved with reservations].
        Wellcome Open Res. 2017; 2: 55
        • van der Kruijs S.J.
        • Bodde N.M.
        • Vaessen M.J.
        • Lazeron R.H.
        • Vonck K.
        • Boon P.
        • Jansen J.F.
        Functional connectivity of dissociation in patients with psychogenic non-epileptic seizures.
        J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2012; 83: 239-247
        • Gray C.
        • Reuber M.
        • Roberts N.
        • Levita L.
        • Myers L.
        Shame in the treatment of patients with Psychogenic Nonepileptic Seizures: the elephant in the room?.
        Submitted to Seizure. 2021;